Exploring subcolony differences in foraging and reproductive success: the influence of environmental conditions on a central place foraging seabird
Abstract
While differences in foraging and reproductive success are well studied between seabird colonies, they are less understood at a smaller subcolony scale. Working with little penguins (Eudyptula minor) at Phillip Island, Australia, we used an automated penguin monitoring system and performed regular nest checks at two subcolonies situated 2 km apart during the 2015/2016 breeding seasons. We examined whether foraging and reproductive success differed between subcolonies. We used satellite data to examine how sea surface temperature, as environmental pressure, in the foraging regions from each subcolony influenced their foraging performance. In the pre-laying and incubation breeding stages, the birds from one subcolony had a lower foraging success than birds from the other. However, this pattern was reversed between the subcolonies in the guard and post-guard stages. Breeding success data from the two subcolonies from 2004–2018 showed that reproductive success and mean egg-laying had a negative relationship with sea surface temperature. We highlighted that variation in foraging and reproductive success can arise in subcolonies, likely due to different responses to environmental conditions and prey availability. Differences at the subcolony level can help refine, develop and improve appropriate species management plans for conserving a range of colonial central place seabirds.
1. Introduction
Limitation in space and food resources is one of the most critical factors contributing to competition in colonial central place foraging seabird populations [1]. Although coloniality is an advantageous adaptation commonly found in seabirds, closely associating with conspecifics can result in density-dependent competition [2–5]. During the breeding season, parent birds are constrained to a central place, which can increase the competition between conspecifics in the area [6–9]. Density-dependent competition affects the growth rate of a colony, with an increasing colony size creating a decline in growth rate [1,10].
The ecological niche theory proposes that resources must be segregated for morphologically similar species to coexist, particularly in space, time or diet [11,12]. When there is spatial segregation of foraging and breeding sites, between colonies or within colonies, the competition for resources can decrease [13–16]. Subcolonies exist when spatial segregation occurs within a colony. These subcolonies may experience a higher level of competition than exists with between-colony segregation, due to conspecifics using more similar resources [17]. Increased competition for resources is also likely to be more evident when there is a higher constraint, typically low prey availability [18]. Other than the competition for resources, factors such as age, sex, breeding status and predation can also lead to the spatial segregation of foraging and breeding sites [19–21].
Understanding how the environment influences seabirds' foraging and reproductive success is imperative [22]. In particular, sea surface temperature (SST) is a good predictor of reproductive success, prey abundance and prey distribution [23–25]. Prey abundance is generally higher during the warmer months when many seabirds use this as a signal to start breeding [24]. However, foraging opportunities can be limited when SST is above the average as prey, such as clupeids, have a low thermal tolerance in these conditions [23,26,27]. Prey distribution can be influenced by stratification in the water column and by differences in SST which can result from factors such as water currents, wind, and tide [28–31]. As SST is quite conservative in space and time, small changes can greatly alter prey abundance and distribution. Even within small regions, these differences are likely to occur at both the colony and the subcolony level [20,32,33].
When prey is limited, seabirds can alternate between short and long foraging trips, prey on more energy-dense species, forage in different areas, and increase their foraging range [34–36]. An animal under poor prey availability and environmental conditions may prioritize their fitness over their chicks’ and abandon a breeding attempt [37,38]. Many studies have investigated the impact of environmental conditions and variation in foraging and reproductive success between seabird colonies (e.g. [16,39–41]). However, information is scarce on these factors at a finer scale within a colony [14,42,43]. Many seabird studies investigate one specific area of a colony due to ease of data collection or technological limitations [29,44,45]. However, if there are differences in foraging and reproductive success within a colony it would identify the need for future studies on central place foragers to be investigated at this finer scale to inform effective conservation management strategies.
The little penguin (Eudyptula minor) is the smallest penguin species and has one of the shortest foraging ranges among seabirds, approximately 20 km or less from their colony during the breeding season [7,46]. This short foraging range poses a challenge for the parents to meet the energy requirements of their chicks and themselves [7]. Within the breeding season, there are four breeding stages; pre-laying (30 days), incubation (35 days), guard (15 days) and post-guard (42 days on average) [47]. One of the largest colonies of little penguin species at Phillip Island, Australia has an estimated 28 000 to 32 000 individuals [48]. Due to intra-specific competition, the colony has undergone a natural division into subcolonies, each occupying different foraging and breeding sites, with little spatial overlap [1,10,43]. Throughout the breeding season, the spatial foraging segregation of these subcolonies can vary from partial to complete. By contrast, if they were separate colonies, there would consistently be complete segregation [43].
We used two subcolonies of little penguin on Phillip Island as a model system to determine their variation in foraging and reproductive success. Given the spatial segregation of the subcolonies at Phillip Island, we further investigated the differences in reproductive success and whether the timing of breeding of each subcolony was influenced by the environmental conditions at each subcolony's foraging area. By expanding on the data set used by [43], we predicted that (1) there would still be sub-colony differences in adult body mass change per day of a foraging trip and also in foraging trip duration over all four breeding stages, (2) there would still be differences in the reproductive success and also in mean laying dates of each subcolony as a result of these foraging differences and (3) that there would be a difference in the sea surface temperature at each subcolony's foraging site which would be an indicator of thermal conditions in the area and this difference would influence the reproductive success and mean laying date of each subcolony.
2. Material and methods
2.1. Study site
We investigated spatial segregation of foraging and breeding sites between two little penguin subcolonies in the mega-colony of 32 000 birds at Phillip Island, Victoria, Australia (38°31′S, 145°09′E) [48]. The two subcolonies are located approximately 2 km apart, with one subcolony at Radio-tracking Bay and the other at Penguin Parade, with approximately 100–150 breeding pairs in each (figure 1). The breeding season for little penguins occurs in the Austral summer, usually between September and February [47]. A breeding pair can lay 1–2 eggs per clutch and have up to three clutches if it is a successful breeding season, but usually only lay one clutch at Phillip Island [47].
2.2. Foraging success and automated data collection
Foraging success was indicated by foraging trip duration and body mass change per day. Foraging trip duration is the number of days an individual spent at sea in one trip. It was calculated by: Day of the year returning to the subcolony – Day of the year leaving the subcolony. Body mass change per day was defined as body mass, to the nearest 1 g, gained or lost by an individual for each day of a foraging trip. It was calculated by: (Mass (g) returning to the subcolony – Mass (g) leaving the subcolony)/Foraging trip duration (days). To determine how foraging success differed between subcolonies, the analytical models included foraging trip duration and body mass change as response variables, and breeding season, sex, age, and location (i.e. foraging site) as predictor variables, with a random effect of transponder number to account for individual differences. Breeding season, sex and age were included in the models because they may also influence the foraging success of a subcolony [20,21].
The data that were used to determine foraging success were collected from the 2015 and 2016 breeding seasons. Each subcolony's breeding site had an automated penguin monitoring system (APMS) installed to detect the movement of birds in and out of the subcolony (figure 1). The APMS at each site was located on the main penguin pathway into the subcolony [49]. Each time the penguin entered and left the colony, it passed through the APMS and the date, time, penguin body mass (g) and ID were recorded [47]. The APMS records when the penguins enter and leave the colony on successive trips (from which foraging trip duration can be calculated) and the mass to the nearest gram before and after each successive foraging trip (from which change in body mass can be calculated). Each APMS was checked and the weighing platform calibrated weekly.
Each record from the APMS was marked as either ‘IN’ or ‘OUT’ to indicate whether the penguin was entering or leaving the colony, respectively. If an individual was recorded in the 12 h before UTC + 15 : 00, the record was marked ‘IN’ and if recorded in the 12 h after, was marked ‘OUT’ to correspond with their typical foraging patterns [47]. Individuals sometimes do not pass through the APMS when entering or leaving the colony, so there were records with consecutive ‘IN’ or consecutive ‘OUT’ values. To account for this, any records greater than 12 h apart with consecutive ‘IN’ or ‘OUT” values had a record with the opposite value added at an equal time between the two consecutive values. The records with interpolated data were only used to analyse foraging trip duration and not used for body mass change because foraging trip duration is generally much more consistent in each breeding stage, while body mass change can be more variable and not as well predicted [29]. Foraging trip durations of ≥15 days were considered outliers and removed [47]. Body mass change per day was limited to between –50 g and 420 g as changes outside this range were also considered outliers [50].
2.3. Reproductive success and nest monitoring
Reproductive success was measured as the number of chicks that had fledged per breeding pair in a single season. Chicks fledged per pair was calculated from the number of chicks fledged in a breeding season divided by the number of breeding females in the breeding season.
We monitored 100 nests at Radio-tracking Bay and Penguin Parade subcolonies throughout the breeding seasons from 2004 to 2018 to collect penguin breeding data. The subcolony at the Penguin Parade was checked three times per week, while the subcolony at Radio-tracking Bay was checked once every two weeks, except for the breeding seasons in 2015 and 2016 when it was checked weekly [43]. Sites were checked at different intervals due to the logistics of accessing different parts of the colony and accommodating various studies. The intervals were still much less than the time the birds spent in any of the breeding stages, so there was little impact on data quality. Penguin Parade had artificial nestboxes and natural burrows, and Radio-tracking Bay had natural burrows only.
Each nest was checked for occupancy at the frequency described for the presence of adults, chicks and eggs. This provided each nest's egg-laying dates, hatching dates and fledging dates. Chicks that were fully feathered and were aged greater than 40 days when last encountered were considered to have fledged [34]. Birds were permanently identified with a passive identification transponder (TIRIS, Texas Instruments, USA, Trovan, Australia and Allflex, Australia) read by a handheld scanner. Newly encountered adults and chicks had a transponder inserted subcutaneously between the scapulae for subsequent identification (see details in [47]). Adults without ID were tagged with a transponder when encountered more than once in the same nest and considered three years old when encountered [51]. Chicks were marked at seven weeks old. The sex of each penguin was determined by bill measurements [52].
2.4. Comparing reproductive success, the timing of breeding and environmental conditions
To determine how reproductive success, the timing of breeding and environmental conditions differed between subcolonies, the number of chicks fledged per pair, the mean laying date and the SST in the foraging sites of each subcolony, respectively, were compared between the subcolonies. To determine the impact of environmental conditions, we compared the number of chicks fledged per pair and the mean egg-laying date for each subcolony with the mean value of SST in the foraging area of each subcolony.
When SST in the waters surrounding the Phillip Island mega-colony increases after the Austral winter, the penguins use this as a signal to start breeding [24]. The lag time between this increase in temperature and the mean laying date is usually about seven weeks [24]. Therefore, SST for each subcolony foraging area was calculated as the average daily value between the 6th and 7th week before the mean laying date for the birds at Penguin Parade from the 2004–2018 breeding seasons [24].
Sea surface temperature was obtained through satellite data from Copernicus Marine Environment Monitoring Service [53]. The boundaries of the foraging areas of each subcolony that were used to obtain SST were determined by Sánchez et al. [43] where the birds from each subcolony were tracked at sea and their foraging areas located. Daily SST was collected from 2004 to 2018 at a spatial resolution of 0.083°. The mean value of SST of each foraging area of birds from Radio-tracking Bay was recorded within 38′S and 40′S and 144′E and 145′E, and from Penguin Parade was recorded within 38′S and 40′S and 145′E and 146′E [43].
2.5. Statistical methods
All statistical analyses were conducted using R 3.5.3 [54] and RStudio 1.0.136 [55]. Foraging trip duration and body mass change were analysed for each breeding stage from the 2015 and 2016 breeding seasons, as these were the only years of breeding stage data available. For this study, only individuals with a transponder number monitored for reproductive success at each subcolony were included in the foraging data.
We used generalized linear mixed-effects models to detect any differences in foraging trip duration between breeding sites (Family = Poisson, Link function = Log). However, the data were highly skewed, so we used a mixed effects cox proportional survival analysis from the R package ‘coxme’ [56]. The 'survival' goal was defined as a penguin returning from a one-day trip versus 2+ day foraging trips. This analysis was performed for each breeding stage because there are different parental requirements for trip duration and the need to feed chicks; therefore, they can be considered independent (figure 2) [47].
The assumption of proportional hazards (1 trip versus 2+ trips) was checked using the R package ‘survival’ [57]. An interaction between location and foraging trip duration was included only in the model for the incubation breeding stage to meet assumptions. No other interactions were observed between the predictor variables (i.e. breeding season, sex, age and location), so they were removed from the model. The full model for the pre-laying, guard and post-guard breeding stages was: Foraging trip duration ∼ Season + Sex + Age + Location + (1| transponder). The full model for the incubation breeding stage was: Foraging trip duration ∼ Season + Sex + Age + Location + Location: Foraging trip duration + (1| transponder). Significance was set at less than 0.05.
To determine the relationship between body mass change and breeding site, a linear mixed-effects model was used to analyse body mass change from the R package ‘lmerTest’ [58]. The assumptions that the residuals were normally distributed, independent, and had equal variances were checked via residual plots and were met. This analysis was performed for each breeding stage as there are different parental requirements in each stage, and therefore body mass will differ in each stage (figure 3) [47]. The full model for each breeding stage was: body mass change per day ∼ Season + Sex + Age + Location + (1| transponder). No interactions were observed between the predictor variables (i.e. breeding season, sex, age and location) so they were removed from the model. Significance was set at less than 0.05.
Model selection was performed for foraging trip duration and body mass change using the ‘dredge’ function from the R package ‘MuMIn’ [59]. The ‘dredge’ function generates a list of models with different combinations of predictor variables. Akaike's information criterion was used to select the best-fit model [60]. The best-fit models had the sum of Akaike weights ≥0.9. From these models, the predictor variables' relative importance weights (RIW) were calculated by the sum of Akaike weights of the best-fit models where the predictor variable was present. The intervals to determine effect were: RIW ≥ 0.9 = strong effect, 0.6 ≥ moderate effect less than 0.9, 0.5 ≥ very weak effect less than 0.6, RIW less than 0.5 = no effect. For interactions, RIW > 0.7 = strong effect and RIW > 0.5 = moderate effect [61].
Paired t-tests were performed by pairing each subcolony, Penguin Parade and Radio-tracking Bay, for the breeding seasons from 2004 to 2018. A paired t-test was performed for the response variables; chicks fledged per pair, mean laying date and SST. The data for chicks fledged per pair from 2004–2015 was sourced from Sánchez et al. [43]. The data from Sánchez et al. [43] were tested using further years of data to compare by year with mean laying date and SST data. The normality of data was checked using Shapiro-Wilks normality tests, and equal variances were checked using Bartlett tests of homogeneity of variances. All assumptions were met. Significance was set at less than 0.05.
3. Results
3.1. Effect of location on foraging trip duration and body mass change
Over the two breeding seasons for both locations, 16 221 penguin crossing records from the APMS were used to analyse foraging trip duration and 5337 crossings were used to analyse body mass change (table 1). Foraging trip duration of one day had the highest frequency in all breeding stages (69% of records in the pre-laying stage, 53% of records in the incubation stage, 92% of records in the guard stage, 87% of records in the post-guard stage) (figure 4).
breeding stage | foraging trip duration |
body mass change |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
total number of records | penguin parade (days ± s.e.) | radio-tracking bay (days ± s.e.) | total number of records | penguin parade (g ± s.e.) | radio-tracking bay (g ± s.e.) | |
pre-laying | 2665 | 2.32 ± 0.06 | 2.05 ± 0.08 | 778 | 125.43 ± 4.91 | 160.68 ± 6.17 |
incubation | 2536 | 2.75 ± 0.06 | 2.32 ± 0.08 | 917 | 131.25 ± 4.42 | 159.60 ± 5.62 |
guard | 4570 | 1.16 ± 0.01 | 1.18 ± 0.02 | 1400 | 273.19 ± 3.77 | 235.45 ± 5.16 |
post-guard | 6450 | 1.17 ± 0.01 | 1.27 ± 0.02 | 2242 | 256.21 ± 2.89 | 203.19 ± 4.37 |
The location had a strong effect on foraging trip duration (RIW = 0.9) and on body mass change (RIW = 1) in all breeding stages, except for foraging trip duration in the guard stage (tables 2 and 3). Season and sex had very weak to no effect on foraging trip duration in all breeding stages, except in the pre-laying stage, where they both had a strong effect (tables 2 and 3). By contrast, season and sex had a strong effect on body mass change in all breeding stages, except for in the post-guard stage where season had a moderate effect. There was no effect of age on foraging trip duration or body mass change (tables 2 and 3). RIW was not calculated for the pre-laying stage, given that a single model explained all the variability of the response variable (table 3).
response variable | breeding stage | best fit models | Akaike weight (%) |
---|---|---|---|
foraging trip duration (days) | pre-laying | foraging trip duration ∼ location + season + sex | 97 |
incubation | foraging trip duration ∼ location + location: foraging trip duration | 54 | |
foraging trip duration ∼ location + sex + location: foraging trip duration | 20 | ||
foraging trip duration ∼ location + season + location: foraging trip duration | 20 | ||
guard | null | 30 | |
foraging trip duration ∼ location | 18 | ||
foraging trip duration ∼ sex | 15 | ||
foraging trip duration ∼ season | 11 | ||
foraging trip duration ∼ location + sex | 9 | ||
foraging trip duration ∼ location + season | 7 | ||
post-guard | foraging trip duration ∼ location + sex | 35 | |
foraging trip duration ∼ location | 27 | ||
foraging trip duration ∼ location + season + sex | 15 | ||
foraging trip duration ∼ location + season | 12 | ||
foraging trip duration ∼ sex | 4 | ||
body mass change per day (g) | pre-laying | body mass change ∼ location + season + sex | 99 |
incubation | body mass change ∼ location + season + sex | 98 | |
guard | body mass change ∼ location + season + sex | 76 | |
body mass change ∼ location + sex | 12 | ||
body mass change ∼ location + season | 11 | ||
post-guard | body mass change ∼ location + season + sex | 83 | |
body mass change ∼ location + sex | 15 |
predictor variable | foraging trip duration |
body mass change |
|||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
incubation | guard | post-guard | guard | post-guard | |
location | 0.9 | 0.3 | 0.9 | 1 | 1 |
season | 0.2 | 0.2 | 0.3 | 0.9 | 0.8 |
sex | 0.2 | 0.2 | 0.5 | 0.9 | 1 |
age | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 |
location: foraging trip duration | 0.9 | NA | NA | NA | NA |
In the pre-laying stage, birds at Penguin Parade had a significantly longer foraging trip duration and significantly lower body mass gain than at Radio-tracking Bay. A significantly lower body mass gain for birds at Penguin Parade also occurred in the incubation stage (tables 1 and 4, A1; figures 5, A1; refer to electronic supplementary material, table A1 and figure A1 in the supplementary material). However, this pattern was reversed in the guard and post-guard breeding stages. In the post-guard breeding stage, the birds at Radio-tracking Bay showed significantly longer foraging trip durations and significantly lower body mass gain than those at Penguin Parade. A significantly lower body mass gain for the birds at Radio-tracking Bay also occurred in the guard stage (tables 1 and 4, A1; Figures 5, A1).
A | z-value | d.f. | p-value |
---|---|---|---|
pre-laying | 2.70 | 35.89 | <0.05 |
incubation | 0.00 | 6.01 | 1.00 |
guard | −0.36 | 58.69 | 0.72 |
post-guard | −2.72 | 110.21 | <0.05 |
B | t-value | d.f. | p-value |
pre-laying | 3.113 | 93.9239 | <0.05 |
incubation | 3.134 | 140.0869 | <0.05 |
guard | −4.973 | 109.6576 | <0.05 |
post-guard | −6.850 | 103.6000 | <0.05 |
3.2. Reproductive success, mean laying date and sea surface temperature
On average, 30% (0.3 ± 0.10 chicks) more chicks fledged per pair for the subcolony at Penguin Parade when compared with the subcolony at Radio-tracking Bay in the breeding seasons from 2004 to 2018 (t-value = 3.0603, d.f. = 14, p-value < 0.05) (electronic supplementary material, table A2; figures 6 and 7; refer to electronic supplementary material, table A2 in the supplementary material). Sea surface temperature was significantly higher in the foraging area of birds from Radio-tracking Bay than the foraging area of those from Penguin Parade over the same period (t-value = −7.4567, d.f. = 14, p-value < 0.05, figure 9) (electronic supplementary material, table A3; figures 6 and 8; refer to electronic supplementary material, table A3 in the supplementary material). Sea surface temperature also showed a negative relationship with each subcolony's reproductive success data (figure 9). There was also a significantly earlier mean egg-laying date for the subcolony at Radio-tracking Bay than at Penguin Parade (t-value = 2.5544, d.f. = 14, p-value < 0.05) (electronic supplementary material, table A4; figures 6 and 10; refer to electronic supplementary material, table A4 in the supplementary material).
4. Discussion
Our study revealed differences in foraging trip duration, body mass change, reproductive success and mean egg-laying date between the little penguin subcolonies. Penguins from the Penguin Parade subcolony took longer foraging trips. They gained less body mass per day than those from the Radio-tracking Bay subcolony during the pre-laying and incubation stages. However, this pattern was reversed between the subcolonies in the guard and post-guard breeding stages. The foraging zones of each subcolony also differed in SST where the foraging area of the birds from Radio-tracking Bay had a significantly higher SST than that of the birds from Penguin Parade. In both subcolonies, the reproductive success decreased with an increased sea surface temperature. Our results show differences in foraging and reproductive success between subcolonies, and differences in the environmental foraging conditions for penguins at each site.
Foraging trip duration and body mass change differed between subcolonies in all breeding stages, except for foraging trip duration in the guard stage. Foraging success depends on prey abundance and distribution [43]. The little penguins in the two subcolonies may have had different success in finding food because they foraged in different areas [43]. Birds may follow favourable prey conditions, so they travel to other areas depending on foraging opportunities [45,62]. Poor prey availability can cause birds to spend more time at sea to find enough food, changing the length of foraging trips and body mass [34]. Foraging differences due to season, sex, and age are observed in a range of seabird species [63–66]. However, in our study, the location of foraging and breeding site had a stronger influence on foraging trip duration and body mass change than the season, sex, and age, showing that different foraging and breeding sites strongly influence foraging success.
Accessibility to other foraging areas under the different breeding stages’ restrictions may result in differences in foraging success, even at this fine scale. The birds from Radio-tracking Bay may travel to more profitable feeding grounds in Port Phillip Bay during the less constrained pre-laying and incubation stages [7,43]. The birds from Penguin Parade are unlikely to travel to Port Phillip Bay, foraging instead in less productive offshore waters [43]. As a result, in the pre-laying and incubation stages, the Penguin Parade subcolony had a lower foraging success than the Radio-tracking Bay subcolony. By contrast, during the guard and post-guard breeding stages, birds at Radio-tracking Bay had a lower foraging success than the birds at Penguin Parade, suggesting that the local foraging grounds (less than 20 km from their breeding site) for the subcolony at Radio-tracking Bay had a lower prey availability than local foraging grounds for the subcolony at Penguin Parade. A more robust explanation, however, may be related to the bathymetry in the foraging areas [43,46,67]. The foraging area for the birds at Penguin Parade is shallower than the foraging area at Radio-tracking Bay. It may explain why the subcolony at Penguin Parade had a higher foraging success than the subcolony at Radio-tracking Bay during chick-rearing which has a higher constraint and fewer options for travelling further [43]. No effect of location on foraging trip duration was observed in the guard stage as birds only make one-day trips at this stage [8,47,68]. No effect was observed in the incubation stage as penguins travel longer distances to avoid local segregation and exploit food resources that will not be reached at chick rearing stages [69].
We detected variations in the success of different little penguin subcolonies on Phillip Island where the subcolony at Radio-tracking Bay had a lower foraging and reproductive success than the subcolony at Penguin Parade. The differing environments in each foraging zone may explain the variation in success. Birds at Radio-tracking Bay could shift to better foraging grounds used by birds at Penguin Parade. However, what keeps the subcolonies separated is the high level of foraging and nest competition that initially caused the segregation [13,15,16]. Although central place foraging seabird species, such as albatross and petrels, can have longer foraging ranges resulting in lower levels of competition than little penguins, these species are still constrained to the central place and are limited in their foraging range [70,71]. Therefore, this study's differences in foraging at the sub-colony level can apply to a wide range of central place foraging seabird species.
We observed differences in SST between the foraging areas of the two sub-colonies, even though they were not too far apart. The foraging area of the birds at Radiotracking Bay had a higher SST than the foraging area of the birds from the Penguin Parade. This could be due to differences in currents, wind or stratification and suggests differences in prey abundance and prey distribution at this small spatial scale [33,61,72]. It may explain the differences in foraging success and reproductive success within the colony. For little penguins elsewhere, a lower SST resulted in a higher probability of foraging in the area and a higher prey capture success [33]. One of the little penguins' preferred prey, sardines (Sardinops sagax), also shows these temperature patterns. Lower SST resulted in higher commercial sardine catch, implying a higher abundance of sardines clustered in areas with a lower SST [23]. A lower SST at the foraging site of the birds from Penguin Parade suggests that there may be a higher prey abundance and better foraging opportunities at this foraging site than at the foraging site of the birds from the Radio-tracking Bay subcolony. As a result, this difference in SST may have caused a higher number of chicks fledged per pair and thus a higher reproductive success for the birds at the Penguin Parade subcolony than the birds at the Radio-tracking Bay subcolony.
On average, birds at Radio-tracking Bay laid eggs earlier in the year than at Penguin Parade. The timing of egg-laying has been associated with the fat reserves of the penguins, primary productivity and SST [24,50,73]. An increase in temperature in early spring triggers the onset of breeding for little penguins at Phillip Island, with the mean laying dates occurring seven weeks after the SST increase, coinciding with the peak of marine productivity (chlorophyll-a concentration) [24], with similar patterns for little penguins in New Zealand [25]. Therefore, the warmer SST at Radio-tracking Bay may result in higher productivity earlier in the year, so the subcolony breeds a week earlier on average due to improved conditions. However, a high SST can limit productivity. This could explain why a higher SST at Radio-tracking Bay and earlier laying dates were also associated with lower reproductive success [26,27]. A similar pattern was found in little penguins near Perth, Western Australia [74].
5. Conclusion
Understanding how environmental conditions affect foraging and reproductive success between subcolonies of other seabird species can help develop and improve appropriate management strategies for conserving a range of seabird species that are colonial central place foragers. Such strategies could include the management of commercial fisheries that may compete with seabird prey resources in the different foraging areas within a colony and the increased protection of areas where subcolonies have low foraging and reproductive success.
Our study shows fine-scale variation within a colony due to high intra-specific competition. We found differences in foraging and reproductive success between subcolonies, likely due to differences in environmental conditions and prey availability in the foraging areas of each subcolony. Overall, our results show how seabirds can respond to small-scale changes in the environment and show the importance of investigating seabird species at the subcolony level to gain fine-scale knowledge that can be incorporated into conservation strategies for a range of colonial central place foraging seabird species.
Ethics
Work was conducted in accordance with Phillip Island Nature Parks Animal Ethics Committee approval number 8.2017 and research permit number 10008474 issued by the Victorian Department of Environment, Land, Water and Planning.
Data accessibility
The raw data and the code used to analyse the data can be found in electronic supplementary material [75].
Authors' contributions
J.P.: conceptualization, data curation, formal analysis, investigation, methodology, visualization, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing; R.D.R.: conceptualization, resources, supervision, visualization, writing—review and editing; A.C.: conceptualization, resources, supervision, visualization, writing—review and editing.
All authors gave final approval for publication and agreed to be held accountable for the work performed therein.
Conflict of interest declaration
We declare we have no competing interests.
Funding
We received no funding for this study.
Acknowledgements
We thank Phillip Island Nature Parks for access to past datasets and for training and support for fieldwork, particularly M. Van Polanen Petel, A. Charuvi and P. Wasiak. Thanks to R. Holmberg for assistance with the APMS, Lige Tan for help with graphs and to all volunteers that helped with fieldwork. Thanks to S. Sanchez for providing much-appreciated advice where needed. Project funding was provided by Monash University and Phillip Island Nature Parks. Work was conducted in accordance with Phillip Island Nature Parks Animal Ethics Committee approval number 8.2017 and research permit number 10008474 issued by the Victorian Department of Environment, Land, Water, and Planning.
References
- 1.
Lewis S, Sherratt TN, Hamer KC, Wanless S . 2001 Evidence of intra-specific competition for food in a pelagic seabird. Nature 412, 816-819. (doi:10.1038/35090566) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 2.
Boulinier T, Mccoy KD, Yoccoz NG, Gasparini J, Tveraa T . 2008 Public information affects breeding dispersal in a colonial bird: kittiwakes cue on neighbours. Biol. Lett. 4, 538-540. (doi:10.1098/rsbl.2008.0291) Link, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 3.
Kazama K, Watanuki Y . 2010 Individual differences in nest defense in the colonial breeding black-tailed Gulls. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 64, 1239-1246. (doi:10.1007/s00265-010-0938-8) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 4.
Sládeček M, Kubelka V, Mlíkovský J, Šálek M . 2014 Coping with nest predation risk in a species-rich bird community inhabiting a Siberian wetland. Folia Zool. 63, 256-268. (doi:10.25225/fozo.v63.i4.a5.2014) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 5.
Jones TB, Patrick SC, Arnould JPY, Rodriguez-Malagon MA, Wells MR, Green JA . 2018 Evidence of sociality in the timing and location of foraging in a colonial seabird. Biol. Lett. 14, 20180214. (doi:10.1098/rsbl.2018.0214) Link, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 6.
Birt VL, Birt TP, Goulet D, Cairns DK, Montevecchi WA . 1987 Ashmole halo - direct evidence for prey depletion by a seabird. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 40, 205-208. (doi:10.3354/meps040205) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 7.
Collins M, Cullen JM, Dann P . 1999 Seasonal and annual foraging movements of little penguins from Phillip Island, Victoria. Wildl. Res. 26, 705-721. (doi:10.1071/WR98003) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 8.
Ropert-Coudert Y, Wilson RP, Daunt F, Kato A . 2004 Patterns of energy acquisition by a central place forager: benefits of alternating short and long foraging trips. Behav. Ecol. 15, 824-830. (doi:10.1093/beheco/arh086) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 9.
Chiaradia A, Forero MG, Hobson KA, Cullen JM . 2010 Changes in diet and trophic position of a top predator 10 years after a mass mortality of a key prey. ICES J. Mar. Sci. 67, 1710-1720. (doi:10.1093/icesjms/fsq067) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 10.
Ballance LT, Ainley DG, Ballard G, Barton K . 2009 An energetic correlate between colony size and foraging effort in seabirds, an example of the Adelie penguin (Pygoscelis adeliae). J. Avian Biol. 40, 279-288. (doi:10.1111/j.1600-048X.2008.04538.x) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 11.
Gause GF . 1934 The struggle for existence. Baltimore, MD: Williams and Wilkins. Crossref, Google Scholar - 12.
Hutchinson GE, Macarthur RH . 1959 A theoretical ecological model of size distributions among species of animals. Am. Nat. 93, 117-125. (doi:10.1086/282063) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 13.
Grémillet D, Dell'omo G, Ryan PG, Peters G, Ropert-Coudert Y, Weeks SJ . 2004 Offshore diplomacy, or how seabirds mitigate intra-specific competition: a case study based on GPS tracking of Cape gannets from neighbouring colonies. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 268, 265-279. (doi:10.3354/meps268265) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 14.
Hipfner JM, Charette MR, Blackburn GS . 2007 Subcolony variation in breeding success in the tufted puffin (Fratercula cirrhata): association with foraging ecology and implications. Auk 124, 1149-1157. (doi:10.1093/auk/124.4.1149) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 15.
Fayet AL, Freeman R, Shoji A, Padget O, Perrins CM, Guilford T . 2015 Lower foraging efficiency in immatures drives spatial segregation with breeding adults in a long-lived pelagic seabird. Anim. Behav. 110, 79-89. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.09.008) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 16.
Delord K, Pinet P, Pinaud D, Barbraud C, De Grissac S, Lewden A, Cherel Y, Weimerskirch H . 2016 Species-specific foraging strategies and segregation mechanisms of sympatric Antarctic fulmarine petrels throughout the annual cycle. Ibis 158, 569-586. (doi:10.1111/ibi.12365) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 17.
Furness RW, Birkhead TR . 1984 Seabird colony distributions suggest competition for food supplies during the breeding-season. Nature 311, 655-656. (doi:10.1038/311655a0) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 18.
Nur N, Sydeman WJ . 1999 Survival, breeding probability and reproductive success in relation to population dynamics of Brandt's cormorants (Phalacrocorax penicillatus). Bird Study 46, 92-103. (doi:10.1080/00063659909477236) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 19.
Webb WC, Marzluff JM, Hepinstall-Cymerman J . 2012 Differences in space use by common ravens in relation to sex, breeding status, and kinship. Condor 114, 584-594. (doi:10.1525/cond.2012.110116) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 20.
Pelletier L, Chiaradia A, Kato A, Ropert-Coudert Y . 2014 Fine-scale spatial age segregation in the limited foraging area of an inshore seabird species, the little penguin. Oecologia 176, 399-408. (doi:10.1007/s00442-014-3018-3) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 21.
Cleasby IR, Wakefield ED, Bodey TW, Davies RD, Patrick SC, Newton J, Votier SC, Bearhop S, Hamer KC . 2015 Sexual segregation in a wide-ranging marine predator is a consequence of habitat selection. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 518, 1-12. (doi:10.3354/meps11112) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 22.
Burrows MT 2011 The pace of shifting climate in marine and terrestrial ecosystems. Science 334, 652-655. (doi:10.1126/science.1210288) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 23.
Yasuda I, Sugisaki H, Watanabe Y, Minobe SS, Oozeki Y . 1999 Interdecadal variations in Japanese sardine and ocean/climate. Fish. Oceanogr. 8, 18-24. (doi:10.1046/j.1365-2419.1999.00089.x) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 24.
Afán I, Chiaradia A, Forero MG, Dann P, Ramírez F . 2015 A novel spatio-temporal scale based on ocean currents unravels environmental drivers of reproductive timing in a marine predator. Proc. R. Soc. B 282, 20150721. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2015.0721) Link, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 25.
Agnew P, Lalas C, Wright J, Dawson S . 2015 Variation in breeding success and survival of little penguins (Eudyptula minor) in response to environmental variation. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 541, 219-229. (doi:10.3354/meps11538) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 26.
Ainley DG 1988 Effects of the 1982–83 El Niño-Southern Oscillation on Pacific Ocean bird populations. Proc. Int. Ornithol. Congr. 19, 1747-1758. Google Scholar - 27.
Bryndum-buchholz A, Tittensor DP, Blanchard JL, Cheung WWL, Coll M, Galbraith ED, Jennings S, Maury O, Lotze HK . 2019 Twenty-first-century climate change impacts on marine animal biomass and ecosystem structure across ocean basins. Glob. Change Biol. 25, 459-472. (doi:10.1111/gcb.14512) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 28.
Takahashi A, Matsumoto K, Hunt GL, Shultz MT, Kitaysky AS, Sato K, Iida K, Watanuki Y . 2008 Thick-billed murres use different diving behaviors in mixed and stratified waters. Deep Sea Res. Part II 55, 1837-1845. (doi:10.1016/j.dsr2.2008.04.005) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 29.
Saraux C, Chiaradia A, Salton M, Dann P, Viblanc VA . 2016 Negative effects of wind speed on individual foraging performance and breeding success in little penguins. Ecol. Monogr. 86, 61-77. Web of Science, Google Scholar - 30.
Weber T, Thomas M . 2017 Influence of ocean tides on the general ocean circulation in the early Eocene. Paleoceanography 32, 553-570. (doi:10.1002/2016PA002997) Crossref, Google Scholar - 31.
Wu ZY, Jiang CB, Chen J, Long YN, Deng B, Liu XJ . 2019 Three-Dimensional Temperature Field Change in the South China Sea during Typhoon Kai-Tak (1213) Based on a Fully Coupled Atmosphere-Wave-Ocean Model. Water 11, 20. Web of Science, Google Scholar - 32.
Ropert-Coudert Y, Kato A, Chiaradia A . 2009 Impact of small-scale environmental perturbations on local marine food resources: a case study of a predator, the little penguin. Proc. R. Soc. B 276, 4105-4109. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.1399) Link, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 33.
Carroll G, Everett JD, Harcourt R, Slip D, Jonsen I . 2016 High sea surface temperatures driven by a strengthening current reduce foraging success by penguins. Sci. Rep. 6, 1-3. (doi:10.1038/srep22236) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 34.
Saraux C, Robinson-Laverick SM, Le Maho Y, Ropert-Coudert Y, Chiaradia A . 2011 Plasticity in foraging strategies of inshore birds: how little penguins maintain body reserves while feeding offspring. Ecology 92, 1909-1916. (doi:10.1890/11-0407.1) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 35.
Jakubas D, Wojczulanis-Jakubas K, Boehnke R, Kidawa D, Błachowiak-Samołyk K, Stempniewicz L . 2016 Intra-seasonal variation in zooplankton availability, chick diet and breeding performance of a high Arctic planktivorous seabird. Polar Biol. 39, 1547-1561. (doi:10.1007/s00300-015-1880-z) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 36.
Kowalczyk ND, Reina RD, Preston TJ, Chiaradia A . 2015 Environmental variability drives shifts in the foraging behaviour and reproductive success of an inshore seabird. Oecologia 178, 967-979. (doi:10.1007/s00442-015-3294-6) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 37.
- 38.
Stearns SC . 1989 Trade-offs in life-history evolution. Funct. Ecol. 3, 259-268. (doi:10.2307/2389364) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 39.
Mancini PL, Bond AL, Hobson KA, Duarte LS, Bugoni L . 2013 Foraging segregation in tropical and polar seabirds: testing the intersexual competition hypothesis. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 449, 186-193. (doi:10.1016/j.jembe.2013.09.011) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 40.
Wakefield ED 2013 Space partitioning without territoriality in gannets. Science 341, 68-70. (doi:10.1126/science.1236077) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 41.
Pratte I, Robertson GJ, Mallory ML . 2017 Four sympatrically nesting auks show clear resource segregation in their foraging environment. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 572, 243-254. (doi:10.3354/meps12144) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 42.
Waggitt JJ, Briffa M, Grecian WJ, Newton J, Patrick SC, Stauss C, Votier SC . 2014 Testing for sub-colony variation in seabird foraging behaviour: ecological and methodological consequences for understanding colonial living. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 498, 275-285. (doi:10.3354/meps10628) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 43.
Sánchez S, Reina RD, Kato A, Ropert-Coudert Y, Cavallo C, Hays GC, Chiaradia A . 2018 Within-colony spatial segregation leads to foraging behaviour variation in a seabird. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 606, 215-230. (doi:10.3354/meps12764) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 44.
Ballard G, Dugger KM, Nur N, Ainley DG . 2010 Foraging strategies of Adelie penguins: adjusting body condition to cope with environmental variability. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 405, 287-302. (doi:10.3354/meps08514) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 45.
Zavalaga CB, Halls JN, Mori GP, Taylor SA, Dell'omo G . 2010 At-sea movement patterns and diving behavior of Peruvian boobies Sula variegata in northern Peru. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 404, 259-274. (doi:10.3354/meps08490) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 46.
Chiaradia A, Ropert-Coudert Y, Kato A, Mattern T, Yorke J . 2007 Diving behaviour of little penguins from four colonies across their whole distribution range: bathymetry affecting diving effort and fledging success. Mar. Biol. 151, 1535-1542. (doi:10.1007/s00227-006-0593-9) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 47.
Chiaradia AF, Kerry KR . 1999 Daily nest attendance and breeding performance in the little penguin (Eudyptula minor) at Phillip Island, Australia. Mar. Ornithol. 27, 13-20. Google Scholar - 48.
Sutherland DR, Dann P . 2014 Population trends in a substantial colony of little penguins: three independent measures over three decades. Biodivers. Conserv. 23, 241-250. (doi:10.1007/s10531-013-0597-y) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 49.
Cavallo C, Chiaradia A, Deagle BE, Hays GC, Jarman S, Mcinnes JC, Ropert-Coudert Y, Sánchez S, Reina RD . 2020 Quantifying prey availability using the foraging plasticity of a marine predator, the little penguin. Funct. Ecol. 34, 1626-1639. (doi:10.1111/1365-2435.13605) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 50.
Salton M, Saraux C, Dann P, Chiaradia A . 2015 Carry-over body mass effect from winter to breeding in a resident seabird, the little penguin. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2, Article No. 140390. (doi:10.1098/rsos.140390) Link, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 51.
Saraux C, Chiaradia A, Le Maho Y, Ropert-Coudert Y . 2011 Everybody needs somebody: unequal parental effort in little penguins. Behav. Ecol. 22, 837-845. (doi:10.1093/beheco/arr049) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 52.
Arnould JPY, Dann P, Cullen JM . 2004 Determining the sex of little penguins (Eudyptula minor) in northern Bass Strait using morphometric measurements. Emu 104, 261-265. (doi:10.1071/MU04035) Crossref, Google Scholar - 53. Mercator Ocean International. 2021 Copernicus marine environment monitoring. See http://marine.copernicus.eu/ (accessed 12 February 2019). Google Scholar
- 54. R Core Team. 2018 R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/. Google Scholar
- 55. Rstudioteam. 2016 RStudio: integrated development environment for R. Boston, MA: RStudio, Inc. Google Scholar
- 56.
Klein JP, Van Houwelingen HC, Ibrahim JG, Scheike TH . 2016 Handbook of survival analysis. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. Crossref, Google Scholar - 57.
Therneau TM, Grambsch PM . 2000 Multiple events per subject. In Modeling survival data: extending the Cox model. Berlin, Germany: Springer. Crossref, Google Scholar - 58.
Kuznetsova A, Brockhoff PB, Christensen RHB . 2017 lmerTest package: tests in linear mixed effects models. J. Stat. Softw. 82, 1-26. (doi:10.18637/jss.v082.i13) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 59.
Barton K . 2009 MuMIn: Multi-model inference. R package version 0.12. 2/r18. See http://R-Forge.R-project.org/projects/mumin. Google Scholar - 60.
Burnham KP, Anderson DR . 2004 Multimodel inference: understanding AIC and BIC and model selection. Sociol. Methods Res. 33, 261-304. (doi:10.1177/0049124104268644) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 61.
Chiaradia A, Ramírez F, Forero MG, Hobson KA . 2016 Stable Isotopes (δ13C, δ15N) combined with conventional dietary approaches reveal plasticity in central-place foraging behavior of little penguins (Eudyptula minor). Front. Ecol. Evol. 3, 154. (doi:10.3389/fevo.2015.00154) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 62.
Zamon JE, Phillips EM, Guy TJ . 2014 Marine bird aggregations associated with the tidally-driven plume and plume fronts of the Columbia River. Deep Sea Res. Part II 107, 85-95. (doi:10.1016/j.dsr2.2013.03.031) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 63.
Weavers BW . 1992 Seasonal foraging ranges and travels at sea of little penguins (Eudyptula minor); determined by radiotracking. Emu 91, 302-317. (doi:10.1071/MU9910302) Crossref, Google Scholar - 64.
Zimmer I, Ropert-Coudert Y, Kato A, Ancel A, Chiaradia A . 2011 Does foraging performance change with age in female little penguins (Eudyptula minor)? PLoS ONE 6, 6. Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 65.
Botha JA, Rishworth GM, Thiebault A, Green DB, Pistorius PA . 2017 Sex-specific foraging over space and time in Cape gannets during chick rearing. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 579, 157-167. (doi:10.3354/meps12299) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 66.
Clay TA, Pearmain EJ, Mcgill RAR, Manica A, Phillips RA . 2018 Age-related variation in non-breeding foraging behaviour and carry-over effects on fitness in an extremely long-lived bird. Funct. Ecol. 32, 1832-1846. (doi:10.1111/1365-2435.13120) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 67.
Meyer X, Macintosh AJ, Chiaradia A, Kato A, Mattern T, Sueur C, Ropert-Coudert Y . 2017 Shallow divers, deep waters and the rise of behavioural stochasticity. Mar. Biol. 164, 149. (doi:10.1007/s00227-017-3177-y) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 68.
Weimerskirch H . 1998 How can a pelagic seabird provision its chick when relying on a distant food resource? Cyclic attendance at the colony, foraging decision and body condition in sooty shearwaters. J. Anim. Ecol. 67, 99-109. (doi:10.1046/j.1365-2656.1998.00180.x) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 69.
Kato A, Ropert-Coudert Y, Chiaradia A . 2008 Regulation of Trip Duration by an Inshore Forager, the little penguin (Eudyptula minor), during Incubation. Auk 125, 588–593. (doi:10.1525/auk.2008.06273) Google Scholar - 70.
Rayner MJ, Hartill BW, Hauber ME, Phillips RA . 2010 Central place foraging by breeding Cook's petrel (Pterodroma cookii): foraging duration reflects range, diet and chick meal mass. Mar. Biol. 157, 2187-2194. (doi:10.1007/s00227-010-1483-8) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 71.
Wakefield ED, Phillips RA, Matthiopoulos J . 2014 Habitat-mediated population limitation in a colonial central-place forager: the sky is not the limit for the black-browed albatross. Proc. R. Soc. B 281, 20132883. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2013.2883) Link, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 72.
Pelletier L, Kato A, Chiaradia A, Ropert-Coudert Y . 2012 Can thermoclines be a cue to prey distribution for marine top predators? A case study with little penguins. PLoS ONE 7, 5. Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 73.
Passuni G, Barbraud C, Chaigneau A, Bertrand A, Oliveros-Ramos R, Ledesma J, Castillo R, Bouchon M, Bertrand S . 2018 Long-term changes in the breeding seasonality of Peruvian seabirds and regime shifts in the Northern Humboldt Current System. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 597, 231-242. (doi:10.3354/meps12590) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 74.
Cannell BL, Chambers LE, Wooller RD, Bradley JS . 2012 Poorer breeding by little penguins near Perth, Western Australia is correlated with above average sea surface temperatures and a stronger Leeuwin Current. Mar. Freshw. Res. 63, 914-925. (doi:10.1071/MF12139) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 75.
Pulvirenti J, Reina RD, Chiaradia A . 2023 Exploring subcolony differences in foraging and reproductive success: the influence of environmental conditions on a central place foraging seabird. (doi:10.6084/m9.figshare.c.6697620) Google Scholar