Abstract
In many animal groups, coordinated activity is facilitated by the emergence of leaders and followers. Although the identity of leaders is to some extent predictable, most groups experience frequent changes of leadership. How do group members cope with such changes in their social role? Here, we compared the foraging behaviour of pairs of stickleback fish after a period of either (i) role reinforcement, which involved rewarding the shyer follower for following, and the bolder leader for leading, or (ii) role reversal, which involved rewarding the shyer follower for leading, and the bolder leader for following. We found that, irrespective of an individual's temperament, its tendency to follow is malleable, whereas the tendency to initiate collective movement is much more resistant to change. As a consequence of this lack of flexibility in initiative, greater temperamental differences within a pair led to improved performance when typical roles were reinforced, but to impaired performance when typical roles were reversed.
References
- 1
Couzin ID, Krause J, Franks NR& Levin SA . 2005 Effective leadership and decision-making in animal groups on the move. Nature 433, 513–516. (doi:10.1038/nature03236). Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 2
King AJ, Johnson DDP& Van Vugt M . 2009 The origins and evolution of leadership. Curr. Biol. 19, R911–R916. (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.027). Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 3
Johnstone RA& Manica A . 2011 Evolution of personality differences in leadership. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 8373–8378. (doi:10.1073/pnas.1102191108). Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 4
Peterson RO, Jacobs AK, Drummer TD, Mech LD& Smith DW . 2002 Leadership behavior in relation to dominance and reproductive status in gray wolves, Canis lupus. Can. J. Zool. 80, 1405–1412. (doi:10.1139/z02-124). Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 5
Rands SA, Cowlishaw G, Pettifor RA, Rowcliffe JM& Johnstone RA . 2003 Spontaneous emergence of leaders and followers in foraging pairs. Nature 423, 432–434. (doi:10.1038/nature01630). Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 6
Fischhoff IR, Sundaresan SR, Cordingley J, Larkin HM, Sellier MJ& Rubenstein DI . 2007 Social relationships and reproductive state influence leadership roles in movements of plains zebra, Equus burchellii. Anim. Behav. 73, 825–831. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.10.012). Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 7
Barelli C, Boesch C, Heistermann M& Reichard UH . 2008 Female white-handed gibbons (Hylobates lar) lead group movements and have priority of access to food resources. Behaviour 145, 965–981. (doi:10.1163/156853908784089243). Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 8
Conradt L, Krause J, Couzin ID& Roper TJ . 2009 ‘Leading according to need’ in self-organizing groups. Am. Nat. 173, 304–312. (doi:10.1086/596532). Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 9
Sueur C, Deneubourg J-L, Petit O& Couzin ID . 2010 Differences in nutrient requirements imply a non-linear emergence of leaders in animal groups. PLoS Comput. Biol. 6, e1000917. (doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000917). Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 10
McClure M, Ralph M& Despland E . 2011 Group leadership depends on energetic state in a nomadic collective foraging caterpillar. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 65, 1573–1579. (doi:10.1007/s00265-011-1167-5). Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 11
Nakayama S, Johnstone RA& Manica A . 2012 Temperament and hunger interact to determine the emergence of leaders in pairs of foraging fish. PLoS ONE 7, e43747. (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0043747). Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 12
Reebs SG . 2000 Can a minority of informed leaders determine the foraging movements of a fish shoal? Anim. Behav. 59, 403–409. (doi:10.1006/anbe.1999.1314). Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 13
Faria JJ, Dyer JRG, Tosh CR& Krause J . 2010 Leadership and social information use in human crowds. Anim. Behav. 79, 895–901. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.12.039). Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 14
Flack A, Pettit B, Freeman R, Guilford T& Biro D . 2012 What are leaders made of? The role of individual experience in determining leader–follower relations in homing pigeons. Anim. Behav. 83, 703–709. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.12.018). Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 15
Harcourt JL, Ang TZ, Sweetman G, Johnstone RA& Manica A . 2009 Social feedback and the emergence of leaders and followers. Curr. Biol. 19, 248–252. (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.12.051). Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 16
Leca JB, Gunst N, Thierry B& Petit O . 2003 Distributed leadership in semifree-ranging white-faced capuchin monkeys. Anim. Behav. 66, 1045–1052. (doi:10.1006/anbe.2003.2276). Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 17
Jacobs A, Maumy M& Petit O . 2008 The influence of social organisation on leadership in brown lemurs (Eulemur fulvus fulvus) in a controlled environment. Behav. Process. 79, 111–113. (doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2008.05.004). Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 18
Stueckle S& Zinner D . 2008 To follow or not to follow: decision making and leadership during the morning departure in Chacma baboons. Anim. Behav. 75, 1995–2004. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.12.012). Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 19
Lewis JS, Wartzok D& Heithaus MR . 2011 Highly dynamic fission-fusion species can exhibit leadership when traveling. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 65, 1061–1069. (doi:10.1007/s00265-010-1113-y). Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 20
Dugatkin LA . 1991 Dynamics of the tit for tat strategy during predator inspection in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 29, 127–132. (doi:10.1007/BF00166487). Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 21
Harcourt JL, Sweetman G, Manica A& Johnstone RA . 2010 Pairs of fish resolve conflicts over coordinated movement by taking turns. Curr. Biol. 20, 156–160. (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.11.045). Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 22
Wolf M, Van Doorn GS& Weissing FJ . 2011 On the coevolution of social responsiveness and behavioural consistency. Proc. R. Soc. B 278, 440–448. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2010). Link, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 23
Beauchamp G . 2000 Individual differences in activity and exploration influence leadership in pairs of foraging zebra finches. Behaviour 137, 301–314. (doi:10.1163/156853900502097). Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 24
Kurvers RHJM, Prins HHT, van Wieren SE, van Oers K, Nolet BA& Ydenberg RC . 2010 The effect of personality on social foraging: shy barnacle geese scrounge more. Proc. R. Soc. B 277, 601–608. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.1474). Link, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 25
Nakayama S, Harcourt JL, Johnstone RA& Manica A . 2012 Initiative, personality and leadership in pairs of foraging fish. PLoS ONE 7, e36606. (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0036606). Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 26
Dall SRX, Houston AI& McNamara JM . 2004 The behavioural ecology of personality: consistent individual differences from an adaptive perspective. Ecol. Lett. 7, 734–739. (doi:10.1111/j.1461-0248.2004.00618.x). Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 27
Borg B, Bornestaf C, Hellqvist A, Schmitz M& Mayer I . 2004 Mechanisms in the photoperiodic control of reproduction in the stickleback. Behaviour 141, 1521–1530. (doi:10.1163/1568539042948213). Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 28
Harcourt JL, Sweetman G, Johnstone RA& Manica A . 2009 Personality counts: the effect of boldness on shoal choice in three-spined sticklebacks. Anim. Behav. 77, 1501–1505. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.03.004). Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 29
Harcourt JL, Biau S, Johnstone R& Manica A . 2010 Boldness and information use in three-spined sticklebacks. Ethology 116, 440–447. (doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.2010.01757.x). Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 30
Kurvers RHJM, Eijkelenkamp B, van Oers K, van Lith B, van Wieren SE, Ydenberg RC& Prins HHT . 2009 Personality differences explain leadership in barnacle geese. Anim. Behav. 78, 447–453. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.06.002). Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 31
Laland K . 2004 Social learning strategies. Learn. Behav. 32, 4–14. (doi:10.3758/BF03196002). Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 32
Pike TW& Laland KN . 2010 Conformist learning in nine-spined sticklebacks’ foraging decisions. Biol. Lett. 6, 466–468. (doi:10.1098/rsbl.2009.1014). Link, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 33
Morgan TJH, Rendell LE, Ehn M, Hoppitt W& Laland KN . 2012 The evolutionary basis of human social learning. Proc. R. Soc. B 279, 653–662. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2011.1172). Link, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 34
Giraldeau L-A& Lefebvre L . 1986 Exchangeable producer and scrounger roles in a captive flock of feral pigeons: a case for the skill pool effect. Anim. Behav. 34, 797–803. (doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(86)80064-1). Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 35
Morand-Ferron J& Giraldeau L-A . 2010 Learning behaviorally stable solutions to producer–scrounger games. Behav. Ecol. 21, 343–348. (doi:10.1093/beheco/arp195). Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 36
Dingemanse NJ& Wolf M . 2010 Recent models for adaptive personality differences: a review. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 365, 3947–3958. (doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0221). Link, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 37
Neuman GA, Wagner SH& Christiansen ND . 1999 The relationship between work-team personality composition and the job performance of teams. Gr. Organ. Manage. 24, 28–45. (doi:10.1177/1059601199241003). Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 38
Mohammed S& Angell LC . 2003 Personality heterogeneity in teams: which differences make a difference for team performance? Small Gr. Res. 34, 651–677. (doi:10.1177/1046496403257228). Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 39
Dyer JRG, Croft DP, Morrell LJ& Krause J . 2008 Shoal composition determines foraging success in the guppy. Behav. Ecol. 20, 165–171. (doi:10.1093/beheco/arn129). Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 40
Pruitt JN& Riechert SE . 2011 How within-group behavioural variation and task efficiency enhance fitness in a social group? Proc. R. Soc. B 278, 1209–1215. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2010.1700). Link, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 41
Modlmeier AP, Liebmann JE& Foitzik S . 2012 Diverse societies are more productive: a lesson from ants. Proc. R. Soc. B 279, 2142–2150. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2011.2376). Link, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 42
Bell ST . 2007 Deep-level composition variables as predictors of team performance: a meta-analysis. J. Appl. Psychol. 92, 595–615. (doi:10.1037/0021-9010.92.3.595). Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 43
Halfhill T, Sundstrom E, Lahner J, Calderone W& Nielsen TM . 2005 Group personality composition and group effectiveness. Small Gr. Res. 36, 83–105. (doi:10.1177/1046496404268538). Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 44
Peeters MAG, Tuijl van HFJM, Rutte CG& Reymen IMMJ . 2006 Personality and team performance: a meta-analysis. Eur. J. Pers. 20, 377–396. (doi:10.1002/per.588). Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 45
Humphrey SE, Hollenbeck JR, Meyer CJ& Ilgen DR . 2011 Personality configurations in self-managed teams: a natural experiment on the effects of maximizing and minimizing variance in traits. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 41, 1701–1732. (doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2011.00778.x). Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar