Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences
You have accessResearch articles

Ants act as olfactory bio-detectors of tumours in patient-derived xenograft mice

Baptiste Piqueret

Baptiste Piqueret

Laboratory of Experimental and Comparative Ethology, Université Sorbonne Paris Nord, Villetaneuse UR4443, France

[email protected]

Contribution: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

Google Scholar

Find this author on PubMed

,
Élodie Montaudon

Élodie Montaudon

Translational Research Department, Institut Curie, 26 Rue d'Ulm, 75005 Paris, France

Contribution: Resources, Writing – review & editing

Google Scholar

Find this author on PubMed

,
Paul Devienne

Paul Devienne

Laboratory of Experimental and Comparative Ethology, Université Sorbonne Paris Nord, Villetaneuse UR4443, France

Contribution: Methodology

Google Scholar

Find this author on PubMed

,
Chloé Leroy

Chloé Leroy

Laboratory of Experimental and Comparative Ethology, Université Sorbonne Paris Nord, Villetaneuse UR4443, France

Contribution: Formal analysis, Investigation, Validation

Google Scholar

Find this author on PubMed

,
Elisabetta Marangoni

Elisabetta Marangoni

Translational Research Department, Institut Curie, 26 Rue d'Ulm, 75005 Paris, France

Contribution: Resources, Writing – review & editing

Google Scholar

Find this author on PubMed

,
Jean-Christophe Sandoz

Jean-Christophe Sandoz

Evolution, Genomes, Behaviour and Ecology, Université Paris-Saclay, CNRS, IRD, 91190 Gif-sur-Yvette, France

Contribution: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing

Google Scholar

Find this author on PubMed

and
Patrizia d'Ettorre

Patrizia d'Ettorre

Laboratory of Experimental and Comparative Ethology, Université Sorbonne Paris Nord, Villetaneuse UR4443, France

Institut Universitaire de France (IUF), Paris, France

[email protected]

Contribution: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing

Google Scholar

Find this author on PubMed

    Abstract

    Early detection of cancer is critical in medical sciences, as the sooner a cancer is diagnosed, the higher are the chances of recovery. Tumour cells are characterized by specific volatile organic compounds (VOCs) that can be used as cancer biomarkers. Through olfactory associative learning, animals can be trained to detect these VOCs. Insects such as ants have a refined sense of smell, and can be easily and rapidly trained with olfactory conditioning. Using urine from patient-derived xenograft mice as stimulus, we demonstrate that individual ants can learn to discriminate the odour of healthy mice from that of tumour-bearing mice and do so after only three conditioning trials. After training, they spend approximately 20% more time in the vicinity of the learned odour than beside the other stimulus. Chemical analyses confirmed that the presence of the tumour changed the urine odour, supporting the behavioural results. Our study demonstrates that ants reliably detect tumour cues in mice urine and have the potential to act as efficient and inexpensive cancer bio-detectors.

    1. Background

    With 10 million deaths, and more than 19 million cases in 2020, cancer is a leading cause of mortality worldwide [1]. A way to increase cancer survival rate is to improve diagnosis methods. The sooner the tumours are detected, the higher are the chances of recovery for the patients. Current early detection methods are often invasive (e.g. coloscopy) and/or expensive (e.g. magnetic resonance imaging); therefore, relatively few people profit from available detection techniques. For example, in France, the most prevalent cancer for women is breast cancer. However, despite France being a relatively rich country, less than half (48.6% in 2019) of the population at risk (women between 50 and 74) underwent screening for this cancer [2].

    A promising method to increase early cancer detection rate is the use of animal olfaction. Trained dogs can detect tumours in cell samples or body-odour samples [3,4]. This method is based on the detection of volatile organic compounds (VOCs) that are characteristic of tumours and linked to their altered cell metabolism [5]. Dogs are not the only animal species used as bio-detectors of cancer. Mice were trained to discriminate tumour-bearing mice from healthy ones [6]. The nematode Caenorhabditis elegans showed attractive chemotaxis to some cancer VOCs [7]. In fruit flies, by using in vivo calcium imaging, an expensive technology, it was shown that olfactory neurons formed specific activation patterns when exposed to the volatiles from a given cancer cell line [8]. Using a well-known and simple paradigm for olfactory conditioning, based on the proboscis extension response [9], honeybees were not able to associate a cancer odour with a reward [10]. It is worth noting, however, that in this same study, dogs were also unable to learn the association, despite the fact that the ability of dogs to detect cancer odours is well documented [4]. Insects are promising detection tools as they are relatively easy to handle, do not require expensive rearing facilities, are available in large numbers and can be trained to recognize an odour in very few trials. Among insects, ants, and especially Formica fusca, demonstrated remarkable learning abilities using ecologically relevant odours. In this species, one training trial is enough to form a genuine long-term memory lasting for days. Furthermore, these ants are highly resistant to memory extinction: after training, they can be tested up to nine times without reward before their responses start to decline [11]. Recently, we tested the olfactory detection abilities of F. fusca towards cultured human cancer cell lines. Using ovarian (IGROV-1) and breast cell lines (MCF-7, MCF-10A and MDA-MD-231) as stimuli, we showed that ants can correctly discriminate between a cancerous cell line and a healthy one, and between two cancerous cell lines [12]. Cell lines have the advantage of being easily available and to provide reproducible odour samples over time, but they do not represent the exact reality of tumours, which are complex tissues composed of different cell types. The use of tumour tissues or whole organisms is therefore a critical step for testing ants as clinical detection tools.

    In this study, we used olfactory stimuli (urine samples) from patient-derived xenograft (PDX) mice carrying human tumours. Compared to cell lines that grow on a stable and known environment (culture medium), PDX mice represent a more realistic model, as the cancerous cells composing a tumour are growing in a live organism with all its complexity. Furthermore, tumours used in PDX are stable in time and can be duplicated, which allows for a virtually infinite number of drug tests and pre-clinical investigations, ensuring that the patient at the origin of the grafted tumour receives the optimal treatment [13,14].

    Here, PDXs were established using a ‘triple-negative’ human breast tumour. Triple-negative tumours are extremely aggressive and patients with this subtype of tumours show poor survival [15]. Urine was chosen as a body-odour stimulus, as it can easily be collected and stored [16]. Ant workers of the species F. fusca were used as bio-detector since they are able to learn and detect cancer-related odours stemming from standardized cancer cell lines [12]. Using a simple conditioning paradigm, we tested whether ants could discriminate the urine of mice carrying a tumour from that of healthy mice. To characterize changes in VOC composition due to the presence of cancer, urine odour samples were analysed using solid-phase micro-extraction (SPME) and gas chromatography coupled with mass spectrometry (GC-MS).

    2. Methods

    (a) Insects and origin of colonies

    Formica fusca is a relatively common ant species in the Northern Hemisphere. Colonies may contain several hundred individuals and are headed by one queen (monogynous) or several queens (polygynous). Three queenright colonies were collected in the forest of Ermenonville (France, 49°09′51.5″ N, 2°36′49.2″ E) and kept under laboratory conditions (25 ± 2°C, 50 ± 10% relative humidity, 12 h/12 h day/night) at the Laboratory of Experimental and Comparative Ethology (LEEC, University Sorbonne Paris Nord). Tested ants were foragers (ants that leave the nest to search for food) and were individually marked with a dot of oil-based paint (Mitsubishi Pencil) on the abdomen and/or thorax 1 day prior to the conditioning. In all cases, each individual ant was used only for one conditioning and test experiment.

    (b) Mice and odour source

    Female immunodeficient mice (Swiss nude mice from Charles River Laboratories) were maintained under specific pathogen-free conditions at the Laboratory of pre-clinical investigation (LIP, Institut Curie, Paris, France). The housing facility was kept at 22°C (± 2°C) with a relative humidity of 30–70%. The light/dark cycle was 12 h light/12 h dark. PDXs from a triple-negative breast cancer (HBCx-11, mesenchymal subtype [17]) were established with consent of the patient. Twelve mice were used in our study: half (n = 6) were graft with tumour at nine-week-old into their interscapular fat pad, the other half (n = 6) underwent the same surgical procedure but without the graft of the tumour (healthy sham group). Mice were kept for seven weeks and were euthanized at the end of the experiment before the PDX had a critical size of 15/15 mm. Tumour size was measured with a manual calliper, and the total volume of a tumour was calculated as V = a × b2/2, a being the largest diameter and b the smallest (table 1 gives details about the size of each tumour).

    Table 1. Size of the tumour of each mouse seven weeks after the initial graft. The estimated volume of tumour (V) was calculated as V = a ×b2/2, with a being the largest side of the tumour and b the smallest side of the tumour.

    identity of the mouse largest side of the tumour (mm) smallest side of the tumour (mm) estimated volume (mm3)
    #1–6 (Sham)
     #11 10 9 405
     #12 12 11 726
     #13 11 11 665.5
     #14 13 11 786.5
     #15 9 7 196
     #16 11 10 550
    mean size 11 9.83 554.83

    Urine of mice was used as olfactory stimuli and was collected seven weeks after the graft. Mice were placed individually in a cage (43 cm × 27 cm, height = 16 cm) without food and water for 3 h. The floor of the cage was covered with filter paper, which was collected at the end of the 3 h soaked with urine. Samples of 1 cm² of urine-imbibed filter paper were inserted individually in an Eppendorf tube and stored at −20°C.

    (c) Experimental protocol

    The experimental protocol was similar to the one used in Piqueret et al. [11]. This protocol was previously implemented using cancer cell-related odours [12], and in addition to providing efficient discrimination between two stimuli, the protocol is simple to teach to a naive experimenter (3 days) and could thus be easily replicated.

    In brief, we trained individual ants to associate an odour (conditioned stimulus—CS, urine of mice) with a reward (unconditioned stimulus, 30% sugar solution) using a circular arena (figure 1a). Three conditioning trials were performed by each ant (figure 2). The time needed by the ant to find the reward was the variable measured in each conditioning trial.

    Figure 1.

    Figure 1. Schema of the experimental device (arenas). (a) Set-up used during the conditioning phase. The time to find the reward was noted. (b) Slightly modified set-up used during the memory tests. The time spent by the ant in the vicinity of the odours (areas, in dashed lines) was recorded during 2 min. Each area measured 35.5 cm2. The orientation of the arena in the experimental room was changed between trials so that ants could not learn visual cues.

    Figure 2.

    Figure 2. Schema of the procedure ants underwent during the behavioural experiments. Individual ants were trained to associate the odour (urine) of mice to the reward (sugar solution) three times during the conditioning procedure and later tested with odours from other mice during the memory tests. Ants were trained either with urine of mice from the sham group (sham as CS, above), or from the tumour group (tumour as CS, below). The red circles indicate that mice have a tumour. During the memory tests, ants had to choose between urine of individuals from the two groups (tumour versus sham). Each ant encountered urine samples from nine different mice during the full procedure. They never encountered the same stimulus more than once to exclude the possible use of individual identity cues.

    To test if ants had learned the CS, we performed memory tests without reward using an arena similar to the one used for the conditioning (figure 1b). In these memory tests, two odorant stimuli were presented at the same time, on each side of the arena. One was the CS, while the other one was a new stimulus (N). When urine from a mouse with tumour was used as CS, we used urine from a sham-mouse as N, and vice versa. Two unscented stimuli were also present (empty Eppendorf tube) to control for the potential learning of visual cues. Circular areas were drawn around each stimulus, which allowed us to record as variable the time spent by the ant in the vicinity of each odorant. Each memory test lasted 2 min and the behaviour of the ant was scored using a behavioural transcription tool (software Ethoc v. 1.2, CRCA, Toulouse, France). All experiments were video recorded with a camera (Canon, Legria HFR806) placed above the experimental arena. Ants underwent three consecutive unrewarded memory tests at 15, 20 and 25 min after the end of the last conditioning trial (figure 2). We used different stimuli (urine from different mice) in the conditioning trials and in the memory test. Therefore, ants could not rely on the possible individual odour of a given mouse between the training and the tests.

    The same stimuli were used for a group of six ants, which is one advantage of using urine stimuli [16]. However, we tested whether significant differences in terms of memory abilities were present when ants were tested with fresh samples (urine never used in a test) or re-used one (used for at least another test). Ants tested with re-used samples did not perform differently from ants tested with fresh samples (linear mixed model—LMM: stimulus × sequence: F = 1.20, d.f. = 2, p ≥ 0.1).

    (d) Chemical analysis

    Odour emitted by the urine of mice used in the behavioural experiments were characterized using chemical analysis. A filter paper (1 cm2) soaked with urine was inserted in a 15 ml glass vial and placed at 37°C using a water bath. A SPME fibre (50/30 DVB/CAR/PDMS, Supelco) was introduced through the PTFE/silicone 9 mm cap of the vial for 50 min [12,18]. After that, the fibre was immediately inserted into an Agilent Technologies 7890A gas-chromatograph, equipped with a HP-5MS GC column (30 m × 0.25 mm × 0.25 µm, Agilent Technologies, Les Ulis Cedex, France). The carrier gas was helium (1 ml min−1), and the injection was splitless (250°C). The oven temperature was programmed at 40°C for 5 min, then increased to 220°C at 7°C min−1 and to 300°C at 15°C min−1 and was held for 3 min. The GC was coupled with a 5975 C Inert XL mass-spectrometer (Agilent Technologies). Mass spectra were recorded with electron impact ionization at 70 eV. Peak areas were integrated with MSD ChemStation software version E.02.01.1177 (Agilent Technologies). Peaks were identified by comparing their ion spectrum to the NIST library (NIST v2.2, 2014) and to standards injected with the same temperature program (benzyl alcohol, acetophenone, nonanal, dodecane, decanal, all from Sigma Aldrich, Saint-Louis, MO, USA).

    (e) Statistics

    (i) Behaviour analysis

    Data were analysed using R software (v. 4.0.0) [19]. Significance was fixed at α = 5%. All data were analysed using LMMs (lmer function from the package lme4) [20]. Normality of dataset was obtained either with log function (for the conditioning dataset) or with square root transformation (for the memory tests dataset). The identity of the ants and the colony were included as nested random factors.

    Acquisition. We analysed the effect of the number of conditioning trials (factor trials) on the dependent variable time (continuous variable, the time to find the reward). We analysed the effect of the conditioning odorant (factor with two levels, tumour-bearing mice or sham mice). We also looked at the interaction conditioning odorant × trials to detect possible differences in ants' responses depending on the odorant used.

    Memory tests. First, we checked whether ants spent more time near the tubes with odours than near the control unscented tubes by analysing the effect of the independent variable presence of odour (factor with two levels, yes or no) on the dependent variable time (continuous variable, the time spent near the odorant vials or the unscented ones). Then, in all experiments, we analysed the effect of the independent variable stimulus (factor with two levels, CS or N) on the dependent variable time (continuous variable, the time spent in the vicinity of a stimulus) during the three memory tests. Finally, we analysed the memory tests separately for each stimulus origin (tumour or sham stimulus).

    (ii) Chemical analysis

    The areas of 49 regularly occurring peaks were standardized by calculating the ln (Pi/g(P)) [21], where Pi is the area of a peak and g(P) is the geometric mean of all the peak areas of the individual. On these variables, we run a principal component analysis (PCA) (electronic supplementary material, table S3) and compared the scores of the first two principal components (PCs) (PC1, explaining 45.57% of the total variance, and PC2 that account for 29.90%) between groups (urine of tumour mice versus sham mice) using Wilcoxon's rank sum test.

    We then calculated the chemical distance of each tumour sample from the centroid of the sham group and we tested whether this distance could be correlated to the size of the tumour.

    3. Results

    (a) Ants can associate the odour of mice urine with a food reward

    We conditioned half of the ants (N = 35, Ntotal = 70) with the urine of tumour-bearing mice and the other half with urine of healthy, sham-treated mice. During the acquisition phase, the ants learned the two different stimuli (urine from tumour mice and from sham mice) in a similar way, as shown by the absence of any significant conditioning odorant × trial interaction (p > 0.1). The time spent by the ants to find the reward decreased significantly across the three training trials (p < 0.001, electronic supplementary material, table S1). This suggests that ants learned that the urine odour (CS) predicted the sucrose reward (figure 3a). Memory tests without reward were then performed to assess if ants could recognize the learned urine type (tumour or healthy).

    Figure 3.

    Figure 3. Ants' performances during acquisition and memory tests in an appetitive conditioning experiment with urine of mice as odour stimulus. (a) Ants were conditioned to associate the odour of mice urine (N = 70) either from the tumour group (N = 35) or from the sham group (N = 35) with a reward (sugar solution 30%). The graph shows the time needed by the ants to find the reward in the circular arena (figure 1). Three conditioning trials were done with an inter-trial interval of 3 min. Significant differences between conditioning trials are indicated with different letters. (b) Time spent by individual ants in the vicinity of each stimulus during the memory tests (circle: conditioned stimulus, CS area; square: novel odour, N area). Circles and squares represent the mean while error bars show confidence intervals (95%). Significant differences between stimuli are noted with asterisks (***: p ≤ 0.001). See also the electronic supplementary material, figure S2.

    (b) Ants can differentiate tumour-bearing mice from healthy ones after a rapid learning phase

    Ants spent more time near the odorant tubes than near the unscented ones (p < 0.001), indicating that ants were not conditioned to the presence of a tube (visual cue), but to the odour. When focusing on the time spent near the tubes with odours, ants conditioned with the urine of tumour mice as CS and ants conditioned with the urine of healthy mice as CS behave similarly, as shown by the absence of significant stimulus × conditioning odorant interaction (p > 0.1, f2 = 0.02). During the memory tests, ants spent more time near the CS odour than near the N odour independently of the odour used during conditioning (p < 0.001, f2 = 0.49, figure 3b). Analysing the two different subsets of data separately gives similar results (ants conditioned with tumour odour: N = 35, p = 0.001; ants conditioned with healthy odour: N = 35, p = 0.006, electronic supplementary material, table S2). These results indicate that ants are able to detect whether mice were sick (presence of a tumour) or healthy based solely on the odour of their urine. In our study, we used a total of 70 ants. As shown above, however, 35 ants are enough to reveal a significant difference between the tumour and tumour-free samples. Based on our results, the minimal number of ants needed is even lower. Indeed, 24 (exact number 23.473) appears to be the minimal number of ants required (power analysis with the following parameters: power = 0.9, Cohen's f2 = 0.49, d.f. = 1, significant level = 0.05).

    (c) Discrimination based on urine volatile organic compounds

    To investigate the cues used by ants to differentiate the tumour mice from the healthy ones, we analysed the urine composition with SPME coupled with GC-MS. The two groups of mice (tumour-bearing and healthy) can be discriminated using PCA. Six PCs with eigenvalues greater than 1, explaining together more than 90% of the total variance (electronic supplementary material, table S3) were used for discrimination. The first two PCs accounted for 75.46% of the total variance (electronic supplementary material, table S3). The two groups (tumour and sham group) are statistically different for the scores of the PC2 (Wilcoxon's test, p < 0.05), but not for PC1 (Wilcoxon's test, p > 0.1). Indeed, in the plot of the first two PCs, the confidence ellipses are partially overlapping each other (electronic supplementary material, figure S1). In particular, the samples from two tumour individuals (#11 and #15) are similar to those from the sham group. We thus investigated whether this could be linked to the size of the tumour.

    In fact, mice #11 and #15 were the individuals with the smallest tumours (respectively, 405 mm3 and 196 mm3, mean tumour size 554.83 mm3; table 1). We then reanalysed the data considering a group of mice with small tumour (below the mean size), and a group with big tumours (above the mean size, table 1). With these new categories, the samples of mice with the big tumours are very well separated from those of the sham individuals (figure 4a; Wilcoxon's test, PC2: p < 0.01).

    Figure 4.

    Figure 4. Chemical differences induced by the tumour presence in mice. (a) Visualization of the categorization of the urine samples using the first two PCs, explaining 75.46% of the total variance. Ellipses represent the confidence interval (0.95). The individuals with large tumour (red) are very well separated from the sham individuals (black). The ones with small tumour (green) are between the two other groups. (b) Relationship between tumour size of mice and chemical distance of their urine samples from those of the sham group. Estimated tumour sizes (x-axis) are plotted against the chemical distance of each tumour sample from the centroid of the sham group (y-axis). Data are highly correlated (Pearson correlation test, r = 0.73, p < 0.1).

    Using the first six PCs (greater than 90% of the total variance, electronic supplementary material, table S3), we calculated the chemical distance (Euclidean distance) of each sample of tumour-bearing mice from the mean coordinates (centroid) of the samples of the sham group (figure 4b). We found a strong link between the size of the tumour and chemical distance from the sham group: the larger the size of tumour, the greater the distance from the sham group (r = 0.73, p < 0.1), although the p-value shows only a tendency given to the sample size (N = 6). Identification of the detected peaks can be seen in the electronic supplementary material, table S4.

    4. Discussion

    We demonstrate that ants are able to learn to associate a complex blend of odours with a reward, after only three conditioning trials. This simple and fast conditioning protocol allowed Formica fusca ants to discriminate tumour-bearing PDX mice from tumour-free PDX mice in unrewarded tests. This discrimination is based on the odour of the urine of mice altered by the presence of a tumour. Using chemical analysis, we also demonstrate that the larger a tumour is, the more different the odour of the bearing individual is from that of the control.

    Formica fusca ants are fast learners and show impressive memory abilities after only three associations between an odour and a reward. Such learning abilities were already described in this species [11], but also in other ant species, like Lasius niger [22], Camponotus spp. [2325] and Linepithema humile [26,27]. Odours used in these studies were pure compounds (e.g. hexanal) or odour blends (e.g. food flavouring), which could be ecologically relevant for ants, as they were based on floral or fruit odours. In our previous study focused on learning of cancer-related odours by ants [12], F. fusca ants were able to associate the odour of cultured human cancer cells (IGROV-1, MCF-7, MCF-10A or MDA-MD-231) with a reward. Obviously, cancer cells are not ecologically relevant for ants, but many animals are known to be able to learn olfactory stimuli that are not present in their natural environment, such as drugs or explosives, given the right incentive [28]. In our previous study on cancer odours, we showed that ants are able to discriminate a cancerous cell line from a healthy one, and a cancerous cell line from a different cancerous cell line.

    PDX mice established with human breast tumours are a step forward in clinical cancer detection and represent an even more complex task for bio-detection as (i) the odour used as stimulus is not directly extracted from the cancer cells, (ii) tumours are not composed of a single cell type and are therefore heterogeneous and (iii) cancer cell odours might be overshadowed by the body odours of individuals or altered when passing from the local micro-environment of the tumour tissue to the body fluids used as conditioning stimulus (e.g. blood, exhaled breath, faeces, sweat or urine). Despite all this complexity, ants reliably discriminated tumour-free urine samples (Sham group) from tumour-carrying ones (Tumour group) independently of the odour used during conditioning.

    Our chemical analysis based on the volatiles emitted by urine, showed that odour samples from the tumour group are chemically different from those of the sham group, and that this difference is correlated to the size of the tumour (the larger the tumour, the more dissimilar the odour of the mouse is from the control). In this study, we used urine from mice with a well-developed tumour (few days before the ethical sacrifice) as odour stimuli. We noted that the level of alteration of the mice odour was linked to the development of the tumour. Based on our chemical analysis, we found that some tumour-bearing individuals were similar to the sham ones. This does not mean that ants were not able to discriminate the small tumour individuals from the sham ones, as ants are able to detect compounds at a very low concentrations [29]. During our behaviour tests, we randomly assigned each ant to the odour of three different mice during the conditioning, and six other mice during the memory tests (three from the conditioned group and three from the other group). In this way, ants could not rely on the possible individual odour of a given mouse. However, this experimental plan was not designed to quantify the impact of a weak stimulus (a small tumour) on the ant performances. It would be interesting to test the olfactory abilities of ants using only small and similar sized tumours, in order to find their detection threshold. From a clinical point of view, the optimal conditioning protocol would have the shorter delay possible between the screening and the result. As a first approach for this question, we analysed separately the results of the tests involving only the small tumours and the tests involving only the large tumours. Independently of the order of presentation, among the 210 tests performed (70 ants × 3 tests), 74 tests involved the smallest tumours (mice #11 and #15, respectively, 405 mm3 and 196 mm3; table 1), and 136 tests involved the largest tumours (mice #12, #13, #14 and #16). No significant stimulus*size interaction was found (LMM: p = 0.5179, F = 0.4189), indicating that the response of the ants was similar between the two groups. We also performed post hoc tests and found that, both for the memory tests involving small tumours and for those involving large tumours, ants were able to discriminate the smell of tumours from the control (LMM: p = 0.0048, F = 8.17 and p = 0.0018, F = 9.85, respectively; electronic supplementary material, figure S3).

    A limitation of our study is that the odours we used may not represent the wide diversity of cancer odours that exists in nature. We trained ants using mice from the same lineage, which were reared on the same controlled diet and received the same PDXs. In a real-life situation, confounding factors such as age, diet, condition or stress may contribute to the inter-individual variability of individual body odours [30]. These factors have been used as criteria for the exclusion of patients in VOCs studies [31]. Future work should thus explore how resilient ants' abilities to discriminate cancer odours are, particularly when they are confronted with a wider diversity of odours and higher levels of background ‘noise’, for instance using mice with varying genetic origins, diets, etc. Furthermore, it would be interesting to investigate if and how F. fusca ants generalize a learnt odour. These ants are able to discriminate cancer cell lines (e.g. breast and ovaries) from each other [12] and could potentially do the same using urine as a source of odour. One could then have ants specialized for the detection of breast cancer, and other ants specialized for ovarian cancer (or in principle any other kind of cancer). The exact composition of the biomarkers emitted by cancer cells is still being investigated and, so far, a clear consensus has not been reached [32], mostly due to the lack of standardization during the sample collection process. Further experiments will reveal whether ants can generalize to all kinds of cancer once trained using a single cancer odour or not.

    In this study, we demonstrated that ants can detect cancer in a whole organism. How could this protocol be implemented in a real-life screening situation? First, we can note that the conditioning/testing method is simple to implement and only requires about 3 days of training to be correctly performed by a naive experimenter. Concerning the conditioning time, on average, approximately 10 min are required to condition a single ant. When adding the three memory tests (performed 15 min after the conditioning), one ant can provide the first discriminating results after approximately 37 min. As reported above, a minimum of 24 ants is needed to reveal a statistical difference between tumour and tumour-free samples. To condition and test these ants, approximately 15 h of work are generally needed.

    As the memory of ants can be updated with a rewarded trial, the conditioning time needed between two patients would be lower than 15 h. This total time could be further reduced, as we previously demonstrated that F. fusca ants have the ability to learn the association of a simple odour stimulus with a reward after a single presentation [11]. We aim at testing this single-trial ability using complex cancer-related odours. Furthermore, more than three tests could potentially be performed by the ants before they need an update of their memory. Indeed, these ants proved to be resistant to memory extinction: after a single conditioning trial, they could be tested up to nine times [11].

    Given that the presence / absence of the disease will be unknown in a real screening test, two tests could be performed for one patient. First, the patient sample (cancer state unknown and to be determined) would be facing a well-known cancer sample. If ants make the difference between the two samples, it would suggest that the patient is cancer free. If the ants do not make the difference, it would indicate that both samples are similar, meaning that the patient might be bearing a tumour. To confirm this result, a second test would be performed, in which the patient sample would be facing a well-known healthy sample. Here, if ants do not express a preference for a sample over the other, it would confirm that the patient is cancer free. If the ants make the difference during this second test, it would suggest that the patient might be bearing a tumour. The coherence of the results in both tests would be an important criterion before proposing further medical diagnostic tests. If the results are not coherent, the two tests should be repeated, up to three times.

    Finally, would it be possible to train not a single ant at a time, but hundreds of individuals at the same time? Social insects (such as ants and honeybees) live in colonies, and communication is essential for them [33]. It was demonstrated that one trained honeybee could transfer a learned association to a naive bee when their antennae were in contact [34]. Studying how information are shared among nest-mate ants could provide an exponentially faster conditioning method if cancer-related odours are shown to be successfully shared within the colony.

    In this first study, combining olfactory abilities of ants and the use of body fluids from human tumours in PDX mice, we found that ants can be used as bio-detectors, as they reliably discriminate healthy individuals from tumour-bearing ones, and vice versa. It is worth noting that human tumours retain their characteristics (heterogeneity, relative proportion of cells and genomic architecture) when grafted on mice [13]. Although promising, this study is a proof of concept. Our method needs further validations using different tumours/cancer types and, most crucially, samples of direct human origins, before being considered suitable as a routine test for cancer screening. All this considered, ants show the potential to become a fast, efficient, inexpensive and non-invasive tool for detection of human tumours.

    Ethics

    The study was approved by the local ethics committee (Breast Group of Institut Curie Hospital). Care and housing of animals were performed with approved criteria from the Institutional Animal Care and French Committee (project authorization no. 02163.02).

    Data accessibility

    The data are provided in the electronic supplementary material [35].

    Authors' contributions

    B.P.: conceptualization, data curation, formal analysis, investigation, methodology, software, validation, visualization, writing—original draft and writing—review and editing; E.Mo.: resources and writing—review and editing; P.D.: methodology; C.L.: formal analysis, investigation and validation; E.Ma.: resources and writing—review and editing; J.-C.S.: conceptualization, formal analysis, project administration, supervision, validation, writing—review and editing; P.d.E.: conceptualization, formal analysis, funding acquisition, project administration, supervision, validation and writing—review and editing.

    All authors gave final approval for publication and agreed to be held accountable for the work performed therein.

    Conflict of interest declaration

    The authors declare no competing interests.

    Funding

    We received funding for this study from Institut Universitaire de France to P.d.E.

    Acknowledgements

    We are grateful to Heiko Rödel for statistical assistance with behavioural analysis. We would like to express our gratitude to Fatima Mechta-Grigoriou for making this collaboration between the LEEC and the LIP possible.

    Footnotes

    Present address: Max Planck Institute for Chemical Ecology, Hans-Knöll-Straße 8, 07745 Jena, Germany.

    Equal contributions of these two authors.

    Electronic supplementary material is available online at https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.c.6392400.

    Published by the Royal Society. All rights reserved.