Adaptive phenotypic plasticity induces individual variability along a cognitive trade-off
Abstract
Animal species, including humans, display patterns of individual variability in cognition that are difficult to explain. For instance, some individuals perform well in certain cognitive tasks but show difficulties in others. We experimentally analysed the contribution of cognitive plasticity to such variability. Theory suggests that diametrically opposed cognitive phenotypes increase individuals' fitness in environments with different conditions such as resource predictability. Therefore, if selection has generated plasticity that matches individuals’ cognitive phenotypes to the environment, this might produce remarkable cognitive variability. We found that guppies, Poecilia reticulata, exposed to an environment with high resource predictability (i.e. food available at the same time and in the same location) developed enhanced learning abilities. Conversely, guppies exposed to an environment with low resource predictability (i.e. food available at a random time and location) developed enhanced cognitive flexibility and inhibitory control. These cognitive differences align along a trade-off between functions that favour the acquisition of regularities such as learning and functions that adjust behaviour to changing conditions (cognitive flexibility and inhibitory control). Therefore, adaptive cognitive plasticity in response to resource predictability (and potentially similar factors) is a key determinant of cognitive individual differences.
1. Introduction
That individuals display different cognitive abilities has been long acknowledged in human psychology (e.g. [1,2]), and evidence is now accumulating for many other animal taxa (e.g. mammals [3]; birds [4]; teleost fish [5]; insects [6]). The pattern of this intraspecific cognitive variability is often quite complex, with individuals excelling at certain cognitive tasks but performing scarcely in others (e.g. [7–12]). Various hypotheses for this cognitive variability have been formulated and tested (e.g. covariation with personality traits [13]; energetic trade-offs [14]); however, we currently do not have a clear explanation.
Intriguingly, the cognitive variability has been observed often in functions that fall into two categories: (i) functions such as learning and memory that are advantageous in predictable environments, addressing consistent patterns and regularities [6,15,16] and (ii) functions (like the executive functions cognitive flexibility and inhibitory control [8,17,18]) that allow individuals to change their behaviour rapidly and are likely advantageous in ever-changing situations [19]. Studies from other research fields have shown that the resource predictability in the environment affects a large number of non-cognitive traits, including foraging behaviour [20–22], aggressive behaviour [23,24], spatial behaviour [25–28], metabolism [21], stress [29], and reproductive and life-history traits [30,31]. For instance, in predictable environments, individuals forage more efficiently [20], are more aggressive [23], occupy smaller territories [26] and display lower levels of stress [29]. If environmental predictability also affects cognitive traits, it might generate interindividual variability along a trade-off between cognitive functions advantageous in highly predictable versus unpredictable environments [32]. Considering that individuals of a species may be subjected to different predictability levels across space and time (e.g. [26,33,34]), a probable mechanism for this effect is plasticity that permits individuals to match their cognitive phenotype to the predictability experienced. This cognitive plasticity would provide a critical contribution to intraspecific variability in cognition.
In our study, we tested the hypothesis that environmental predictability determines cognitive variability via cognitive plasticity. We manipulated the temporal [35] and spatial predictability [20] of foraging resources in experimental populations of guppies, Poecilia reticulata, a teleost fish with heightened cognitive variability (e.g. [18]). The treatment exposed guppies to simulated environments with either a predictable food source available each day in the same place and at the same time or to an unpredictable food source available at a pseudo-random location and time. We then compared guppies exposed to the two treatments using assays for learning, cognitive flexibility and inhibitory control. Based on the aforementioned trade-off hypothesis [32], we predicted a greater learning performance in guppies from the predictable treatment and a greater performance in the cognitive flexibility and inhibitory control tasks in guppies exposed to the unpredictable treatment.
We additionally administered two behavioural tests to the guppies because environmental predictability may affect various behavioural traits in fish (e.g. [21,23,28]), and the behavioural type of a fish often covaries with its cognitive abilities or affects the outcome of some cognitive tests (e.g. [36,37]). Therefore, eventual cognitive differences between the two predictability treatments could be at least in part due changes in guppies' behaviour. By simultaneously characterizing the cognitive and behavioural phenotype of the experimental guppies, we tried to disentangle the mechanism with which predictability affects cognition. We focused on two behavioural traits that have been consistently shown to covary with cognition in guppies: exploration and social behaviour [36,38–40]. Both exploration and sociability are expected to be reduced in the predictable environment [23,26].
2. Materials and methods
(a) Experimental manipulation of resource predictability
The experiments involved naïve, new-born guppies obtained from gravid females in our facility (electronic supplementary material, S1, §a). These subjects underwent exposure to environments with different levels of predictability for 20 consecutive days. We assigned a randomly chosen group of six individuals to each of 12 experimental aquaria (n = 72 guppies overall). Six experimental aquaria were assigned the predictable environment treatment and the remaining six experimental aquaria to the unpredictable environment treatment (n = 6 replicates).
The experimental aquaria were rectangular and contained four separate foraging areas, one in each corner (figure 1a). We administered food to the guppies once per day in one feeding area of the experimental aquarium, with a different schedule for the two treatments. For half of the aquaria assigned to the predictable environment treatment, we provided the food at 11.00 h; for the remaining half in the aquaria of the predictable environment treatment, we provided the food at 15.00 h. Moreover, in the predictable environment treatment, the food was consistently administered in a predetermined foraging area per each aquarium. In the unpredictable environment treatment, we provided the food each day at a random time between 8.00 and 18.00 h and in a foraging area determined according to a pseudo-random scheme. Details of the treatment are provided in electronic supplementary material, S1, §b. At the end of the treatment, four subjects randomly collected from each experimental aquarium were used in the cognitive and behavioural assays, which were administered to a predetermined sequence (electronic supplementary material, S1, §c). We interrupted the testing of one subject because it showed signs of distress in the first assay. Therefore, the sample used in the study was n = 47 guppies, including 23 of the predictable environment treatment and 24 of the unpredictable environment treatment.
(b) Learning assay
The learning assay was based on an established discrimination paradigm in which the guppies had to select a rewarded colour stimulus between two options [41,42]. Briefly, each subject was tested in an experimental apparatus consisting of two chambers connected by a central corridor (figure 1b). The apparatus was maintained under standard conditions and was provided with several enrichments (electronic supplementary material, S1, §d). After a pre-test procedure (electronic supplementary material, S1, §d), each subject underwent 12 colour discrimination trials per day. In each trial, the experimenter inserted two stimulus cards in one of the two chambers of the apparatus. Each card had a circle (∅ 1.8 cm), either yellow or red. For each subject, one of the two colours was considered as the correct stimulus. The correct colour was counterbalanced between the experimental groups and the left-right position of the correct colour was counterbalanced between trials. If the fish approached the correct colour within 15 min, it received a food reward; otherwise, the experimenter removed the cards. As the approach, we considered when the subject swam at 0.5 × body length or less from the stimulus, oriented toward the stimulus. The testing of each guppy continued until it reached a criterion of less than 30% errors 2 consecutive days. In each day of testing, we recorded the number of errors and the number of correct responses of each subject.
(c) Cognitive flexibility assay
After the learning task, we administered a reversal learning task to assess cognitive flexibility following the paradigm of previous studies in teleost fish [42,43]. The apparatus and the procedure were the same as for the test phase of the learning task. However, the experimenter rewarded the choice of the previously unrewarded colour. The reversal learning task started the day after the subject reached the criterion of the learning task. The testing continued until each subject reached a criterion less than 30% errors, and in each day, we collected the number of errors and correct responses, as previously described.
(d) Inhibitory control assay
Following a paradigm implemented in this species [44,45], we assessed inhibitory control as the ability to withhold attempts to capture an unreachable prey behind a transparent barrier. Guppies underwent the inhibitory control assay individually in 4 l experimental aquaria maintained under standard conditions (figure 1c; electronic supplementary material, S1, §e). To perform the assay, the experimenter presented to the subject a laboratory glass tube (∅ 1.2 cm) containing a solution of water and approximately 500 live Artemia salina nauplii. The tube was suspended near one extremity of the apparatus. The guppies were accustomed to A. salina nauplii, as this prey was provided during the maintenance. Moreover, during a pre-test phase, the subjects were trained to feed in correspondence of the same extremity of the apparatus subsequentially used to present the tube (electronic supplementary material, S1, §e). Therefore, most of the subjects rapidly approached the tube and attempted to capture the prey. Guppies’ behaviour was videorecorded for 20 min, allowing the experimenter to record the capture attempts from the recordings played back at a reduced speed. The experimenter recorded as an error each event in which a guppy touched the glass tube with the snout in an attempt to capture a prey. Moreover, the experimenter recorded when the subject approached the stimulus for the first time. Because of an issue with the webcam software, we did not retrieve the recordings of four subjects. Therefore, the sample size of this assay was 43 guppies, 22 of the predictable environment treatment and 21 of the unpredictable environment treatment.
(e) Behavioural tests
First, we conducted a novel environment exploration test in an open-field arena [46–48]. The guppies were observed individually in an unfamiliar, white, empty arena (figure 1d) for 20 min. During this period, using an automatic tracking system (electronic supplementary material, S1, §f), we measured two variables. The first variable was the exploratory activity of the subject as the distance moved. The second variable was the time the subjects spent in the centre of the arena (10 cm from the edges). This latter variable is considered proxy for various behavioural traits. For instance, shyer, more neophobic, and more anxious individuals tend to avoid the centre of the apparatus where they perceive to be more exposed to potential predators (thigmotaxis behaviour [49,50]).
Second, we conducted a social behaviour test [51] in which the guppies were tested in the central compartment of a three-chamber apparatus (figure 1e). The two lateral chambers were divided from the central one by a transparent partition. One lateral chamber contained a shoal of five guppies and the other was left empty. The experimenter released the fish in the apparatus and then recorded its behaviour for 20 min. From the video recordings, the experimenter obtained the time spent by the subject close to the social stimulus (i.e. within 5 cm) and the time spent close to the empty lateral chamber. From these data, we calculated an index of sociability for each subject considering that more social individuals were expected to spend more time close to the stimulus compared to the empty chamber. Further details of this procedure are reported in the electronic supplementary material, S1, §g.
3. Results
(a) Predictable environment enhanced learning
All the subjects tested (n = 47) reached the criterion in the colour discrimination learning task in a relatively short period of time (4.43 ± 2.58 days, mean ± s.d.). The analysis on the number of errors across testing days found a significant decrease (generalized linear mixed-effects model, GLMM: , p < 0.001). This suggested that the subjects progressively learned to choose the correct colour. Critically, the decrease in number of errors was steeper for the guppies of the predictable environment comparing to the guppies of the unpredictable environment (GLMM: , p < 0.001; figure 2a). Therefore, learning was faster for the guppies of the predictable environment.
(b) Unpredictable environment enhanced cognitive flexibility
All the subjects tested (n = 47) reached the criterion in the reversal learning assay. The number of days to the criterion in the reversal learning assay (8.36 ± 4.06 days) was approximately twice as that observed in the initial learning assay, suggesting greater difficulty of the cognitive flexibility assay for the guppies (paired-samples t-test: t46 = 5.126, p < 0.001).
The number of errors in the reversal learning assay significantly decreased across testing days (GLMM: , p < 0.001), as expected due to subject's learning to handle the reversed reward contingency. Critically, the decrease in number of errors was steeper for the guppies of the unpredictable environment comparing to the guppies of the predictable environment (GLMM: , p < 0.001; figure 2b), suggesting greater cognitive flexibility in the guppies of the former treatment.
(c) Unpredictable environment enhanced inhibitory control
Seventy-seven out of 43 guppies attempted to capture the stimulus prey, on average within the third minute (± 3.88, s.d.) from the beginning of the test. The environment experienced by the subject did not affect whether it approached the prey (predictable environment: 16 out of 22 subjects; unpredictable environment: 11 out of 22 subjects; Fisher exact test: = 0.215). Similarly, the environmental treatment experienced did not affect when the subjects approached the prey (two-sample t-test: t25 = 0.099, p = 0.922; figure 3a).
Overall, we observed 1661 attempts to capture the prey, with an average of 61.52 ± 63.39 (mean ± s.d.) attempts per subject. The number of attempts was higher at the beginning of the experiment and then, decreased over testing time (GLMM: , p < 0.001). Guppies from the predictable environment displayed a higher number of attempts (predictable environment: 74.69 ± 67.45; unpredictable environment: 42.36 ± 62.87; GLMM: , p = 0.020; figure 3b).
(d) Level of predictability did not affect variance within experimental group
The performance variance observed within each of the two experimental groups of guppies was not significantly different in any of the cognitive tasks (Bartlett tests: number of errors in the learning assay: Bartlett's K2 = 0.273, p = 0.601; number of errors in the cognitive flexibility assay: Bartlett's K2 = 0.033, p = 0.856; number of number of attempts in the inhibitory control assay: Bartlett's K2 = 0.056, p = 0.813).
(e) Predictability affected guppies' activity but this did not explain cognitive plasticity
In the open-field test, the exploratory activity of the subjects decreased significantly across testing time (linear mixed-effects model, LMM: , p < 0.001). Moreover, guppies from the unpredictable environment showed greater activity (LMM: , p = 0.033; figure 4a). The time spent in the centre of the arena (boldness) in the open-field test was significantly affected only by the experimental time (LMM: , p = 0.008), with no changes due to the treatment (figure 4b). In the sociability test, none of the terms in the model, including the treatment, significantly explained the preference for the social stimulus (LMM: all p-values > 0.5; figure 4c).
A regression analysis indicated that activity did not explain performance in the learning (linear regression: t = −0.740, p = 0.463, R2 = 0.012), cognitive flexibility (linear regression: t = −0.363, p = 0.718, R2 = 0.003) and inhibitory control task (linear regression: t = 0.207, p = 0.838, R2 = 0.002). Therefore, the effects of environmental predictability in the cognitive tasks are not explained by the change in behavioural activity detected in the exploration task.
4. Discussion
Our study revealed that guppies can develop a highly diversified cognitive phenotype that matches the resource predictability level experienced in the environment. When food was predictably found in the same spatial location and at the same time of the day, guppies developed greater learning performance. Conversely, when the location and timing of the food were unpredictable, guppies developed greater cognitive flexibility and greater inhibitory control.
We designed the study to investigate the effect of predictability experimentally, and thus, we analysed populations of subjects exposed to very different levels of predictability. In nature, smaller fluctuations in the predictability levels experienced are likely to similarly determine plasticity-mediated cognitive variability between and within populations. The effect of predictability aligns with growing reports of cognitive plasticity in teleost fishes in response to other environmental factors (e.g. environment quality [52]; social environment [53]; enrichment [42]; predation risk [54]). All these forms of cognitive plasticity may interact in nature, thereby determining a broad spectrum of individual phenotypes. Concerning this, it will be important to ascertain whether the cognitive variability due to plasticity is stable across an individual's life or it can be altered if the environment changes. Some forms of cognitive plasticity displayed by fish are likely malleable, such as those determined by factors that vary with the season [55,56], further amplifying the potential of plasticity to produce individual differences in cognition. Notably, while the teleost's brain certainly displays a remarkable level of neural plasticity potential (reviewed in [57]), including extensive neurogenesis capacity in the adult (reviewed in [58]), cognitive plasticity might be also widespread in tetrapod vertebrates (e.g. [59,60]).
The plasticity due to predictability level has differently affected the three cognitive functions investigated in this study, aligning with the trade-off hypothesis proposed by Tello-Ramos et al. [32]. This provides support for the idea that the observed cognitive plasticity is adaptive at least from two points of view. First, the trade-off hypothesis is based on the fact that enhancing functions such as learning and memory should be advantageous when the environmental conditions are predictable because they permit to rapidly and reliably exploit resources that are available consistently with the same spatial and/or temporal pattern. Conversely, executive functions such as cognitive flexibility and inhibitory control are involved in modifying individuals' behaviour [19], which should permit to adjust to resources that vary in space and time. Second, the function specificity per se supports the idea of an adaptive mechanism. A more general, non-adaptive mechanism is indeed expected to determine unidirectional changes (i.e. an increase or decrease) in all the cognitive abilities of an individual, a scenario that contrasts with what was observed in our study. Function specificity has also been reported for other forms of cognitive plasticity. For instance, guppies raised in the presence of biotic and abiotic stimuli developed greater learning ability compared to guppies raised in barren environments, but no differences were observed in inhibitory control and cognitive flexibility [42]. Many earlier investigations on plasticity and adaptive selection have focused on general proxies of cognition, such as brain size (e.g. [61,62]). The findings in relation to function-specific effects suggest the need of a more precise approach that investigates cognition at the level of single functions to depict cognitive adaptation fully.
The unpredictable treatment also increased guppies’ exploratory activity (but did not affect our measures of boldness and social behaviour). Variability in behavioural traits has been linked to cognitive variability [13], including in our study species [11,36,37,63]. However, in our correlation analysis, the observed effect on activity did not emerge as a potential explanation for the differences observed in the cognitive tasks. Unpredictable environmental conditions are also known to increase corticosteroid concentrations in all main vertebrate groups, including teleost fish (review by [64]), and these hormones may affect cognitive abilities in a range of tasks [65–67]. Therefore, corticosteroids are proximate mechanisms worth investigating for the effect observed in guppies.
In conclusion, we demonstrated a form of cognitive plasticity driven by the levels of predictability of resources in an environment. This plasticity determined variability along a trade-off line between functions useful to learn and fixate a specific behaviour and functions that permit changes in behaviour, highlighting that cognitive plasticity might have an important role in determining phenotypic variance. To fully understand cognitive variability in animals, research efforts should be devoted to analyse other forms of cognitive plasticity and their interacting effects on individual's cognition. Altogether, multiple plasticity mechanisms might be responsible for individualities in cognitive abilities.
Ethics
The experiments complied with Directive 2010/63/EU of the European Parliament and of the Council of 22 September 2010 on the protection of animals used for scientific purposes and with Italian law D. Lgs n. 26 of 4 March 2014 ('Attuazione della direttiva 2010/63/UE sulla protezione degli animali utilizzati a fini scientifici'). The procedures were designed following the ASAB/ABS Guidelines for the Use of Animals in Research (https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2019.11.002) [68] and were approved by the Ethical committee (OPBA) of University of Ferrara (permit TLX-2022-1).
Data accessibility
The data are provided in the electronic supplementary material [69].
Authors' contributions
T.L.-X.: conceptualization, formal analysis and writing—original draft; G.M.: data curation, formal analysis, investigation, methodology and writing—review and editing; C.B.: conceptualization and writing—review and editing.
All authors gave final approval for publication and agreed to be held accountable for the work performed therein.
Conflict of interest declaration
We declare we have no competing interests.
Funding
Work supported by #NEXTGENERATIONEU (NGEU) and funded by the Ministry of University and Research (MUR), National Recovery and Resilience Plan (NRRP), project MNESYS (PE0000006), 'A multiscale integrated approach to the study of the nervous system in health and disease' (DN. 1553 11.10.2022).
Acknowledgements
We are thankful to Andrea Margutti for building the apparatuses, to Roberto Bianchi for help in testing the subjects and to Elia Gatto for suggestions on the statistical analysis.
References
- 1.
Gustafsson JE, Undheim JO . 1996 Individual differences in cognitive functions. In Handbook of educational psychology (edsBerliner DC, Calfee RC ), pp. 186-242. New York, NY: Prentice Hall. Google Scholar - 2.
Humphreys LG . 1979 The construct of general intelligence. Intelligence 3, 105-120. (doi:10.1016/0160-2896(79)90009-6) Crossref, Google Scholar - 3.
Beran MJ, Hopkins WD . 2018 Self-control in chimpanzees relates to general intelligence. Curr. Biol. 28, 574-579. (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2017.12.043) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 4.
Langley EJ, Adams G, Beardsworth CE, Dawson DA, Laker PR, van Horik JO, Whiteside MA, Wilson AJ, Madden JR . 2020 Heritability and correlations among learning and inhibitory control traits. Behav. Ecol. 31, 798-806. (doi:10.1093/beheco/araa029) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 5.
Lucon-Xiccato T, Bisazza A . 2017 Individual differences in cognition among teleost fishes. Behav. Processes 141, 184-195. (doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2017.01.015) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 6.
Mery F, Belay AT, So AKC, Sokolowski MB, Kawecki TJ . 2007 Natural polymorphism affecting learning and memory in Drosophila. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 104, 13 051-13 055. (doi:10.1073/pnas.0702923104) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 7.
Bebus SE, Small TW, Jones BC, Elderbrock EK, Schoech SJ . 2016 Associative learning is inversely related to reversal learning and varies with nestling corticosterone exposure. Anim. Behav. 111, 251-260. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.10.027) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 8.
Bensky MK, Bell AM . 2020 Predictors of individual variation in reversal learning performance in three-spined sticklebacks. Anim. Cogn. 23, 925-938. (doi:10.1007/s10071-020-01399-8) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 9.
Ferrari MC . 2014 Short-term environmental variation in predation risk leads to differential performance in predation-related cognitive function. Anim. Behav. 95, 9-14. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.06.001) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 10.
Kim AE, Oines L, Miyake A . 2018 Individual differences in verbal working memory underlie a tradeoff between semantic and structural processing difficulty during language comprehension: an ERP investigation. J. Exp. Psychol. 44, 406-420. (doi:10.1037/xlm0000457) Google Scholar - 11.
Lucon-Xiccato T, Dadda M . 2017 Personality and cognition: sociability negatively predicts shoal size discrimination performance in guppies. Front. Psychol. 8, 1118. (doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01118) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 12.
Mazza V, Eccard JA, Zaccaroni M, Jacob J, Dammhahn M . 2018 The fast and the flexible: cognitive style drives individual variation in cognition in a small mammal. Anim. Behav. 137, 119-132. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2018.01.011) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 13.
Carere C, Locurto C . 2011 Interaction between animal personality and animal cognition. Cur. Zool. 57, 491-498. (doi:10.1093/czoolo/57.4.491) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 14.
Kotrschal A, Rogell B, Bundsen A, Svensson B, Zajitschek S, Brännström I, Immler S, Maklakov AA, Kolm N . 2013 Artificial selection on relative brain size in the guppy reveals costs and benefits of evolving a larger brain. Curr. Biol. 23, 168-171. (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2012.11.058) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 15.
Carter AJ, Marshall HH, Heinsohn R, Cowlishaw G . 2014 Personality predicts the propensity for social learning in a wild primate. PeerJ 2, e283. (doi:10.7717/peerj.283) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 16.
Ehlinger TJ . 1989 Learning and individual variation in bluegill foraging: habitat-specific techniques. Anim. Behav. 38, 643-658. (doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(89)80010-7) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 17.
Laschober M, Mundry R, Huber L, Schwing R . 2021 Kea (Nestor notabilis) show flexibility and individuality in within-session reversal learning tasks. Anim. Cogn. 24, 1339-1351. (doi:10.1007/s10071-021-01524-1) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 18.
Lucon-Xiccato T, Bisazza A, Bertolucci C . 2020 Guppies show sex and individual differences in the ability to inhibit behaviour. Anim. Cogn. 23, 535-543. (doi:10.1007/s10071-020-01357-4) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 19.
Diamond A . 2013 Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135. (doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 20.
Grand TC, Grant JW . 1994 Spatial predictability of resources and the ideal free distribution in convict cichlids, Cichlasoma nigrofasciatum. Anim. Behav. 48, 909-919. (doi:10.1006/anbe.1994.1316) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 21.
Sloat MR, Reeves GH . 2014 Demographic and phenotypic responses of juvenile steelhead trout to spatial predictability of food resources. Ecology 95, 2423-2433. (doi:10.1890/13-1442.1) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 22.
Stephens DW . 1993 Learning and behavioral ecology: incomplete information and environmental predictability. In Insect learning (edsPapaj DR, Lewis AC ), pp. 195-218. Boston, MA: Springer. Crossref, Google Scholar - 23.
Goldberg JL, Grant JW, Lefebvre L . 2001 Effects of the temporal predictability and spatial clumping of food on the intensity of competitive aggression in the Zenaida dove. Behav. Ecol. 12, 490-495. (doi:10.1093/beheco/12.4.490) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 24.
Grand TC, Grant JW . 1994 Spatial predictability of food influences its monopolization and defence by juvenile convict cichlids. Anim. Behav. 47, 91-100. (doi:10.1006/anbe.1994.1010) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 25.
Cama A, Abellana R, Christel I, Ferrer X, Vieites DR . 2012 Living on predictability: modelling the density distribution of efficient foraging seabirds. Ecography 35, 912-921. (doi:10.1111/j.1600-0587.2011.06756.x) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 26.
Eide NE, Jepsen JU, Prestrud PÅL . 2004 Spatial organization of reproductive Arctic foxes Alopex lagopus: responses to changes in spatial and temporal availability of prey. J. Anim. Ecol. 73, 1056-1068. (doi:10.1111/j.0021-8790.2004.00885.x) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 27.
López-López P, García-Ripollés C, Urios V . 2014 Food predictability determines space use of endangered vultures: implications for management of supplementary feeding. Ecol. Appl. 24, 938-949. (doi:10.1890/13-2000.1) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 28.
Riotte-Lambert L, Matthiopoulos J . 2020 Environmental predictability as a cause and consequence of animal movement. Trends Ecol. Evol. 35, 163-174. (doi:10.1016/j.tree.2019.09.009) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 29.
Gottlieb DH, Coleman K, McCowan B . 2013 The effects of predictability in daily husbandry routines on captive rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 143, 117-127. (doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2012.10.010) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 30.
Webb DG, Marcotte BM . 1984 Resource predictability and reproductive strategy in Tisbe cucumariae Humes (Copepoda: Harpacticoida). J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 77, 1-10. (doi:10.1016/0022-0981(84)90046-7) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 31.
Zammuto RM, Millar JS . 1985 Environmental predictability, variability, and Spermophilus columbianus life history over an elevational gradient. Ecology 66, 1784-1794. (doi:10.2307/2937374) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 32.
Tello-Ramos MC, Branch CL, Kozlovsky DY, Pitera AM, Pravosudov VV . 2019 Spatial memory and cognitive flexibility trade-offs: to be or not to be flexible, that is the question. Anim. Behav. 147, 129-136. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2018.02.019) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 33.
Menge BA . 1972 Foraging strategy of a starfish in relation to actual prey availability and environmental predictability. Ecol. Monogr. 42, 25-50. (doi:10.2307/1942229) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 34.
Minckley RL, Cane JH, Kervin L, Roulston TH . 1999 Spatial predictability and resource specialization of bees (Hymenoptera: Apoidea) at a superabundant, widespread resource. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 67, 119-147. (doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.1999.tb01933.x) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 35.
Bassett L, Buchanan-Smith HM . 2007 Effects of predictability on the welfare of captive animals. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 102, 223-245. (doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2006.05.029) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 36.
Savaşçı BB, Lucon-Xiccato T, Bisazza A . 2021 Ontogeny and personality affect inhibitory control in guppies, Poecilia reticulata. Anim. Behav. 180, 111-121. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2021.08.013) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 37.
Trompf L, Brown C . 2014 Personality affects learning and trade-offs between private and social information in guppies, Poecilia reticulata. Anim. Behav. 88, 99-106. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.11.022) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 38.
Lucon-Xiccato T, Dadda M . 2017 Personality and cognition: sociability negatively predicts shoal size discrimination performance in guppies. Front. Psychol. 1118. (doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01118) Google Scholar - 39.
Lucon-Xiccato T, Montalbano G, Bertolucci C . 2020 Personality traits covary with individual differences in inhibitory abilities in 2 species of fish. Cur. Zool. 66, 187-195. (doi:10.1093/cz/zoz039) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 40.
Mair A, Lucon-Xiccato T, Bisazza A . 2021 Guppies in the puzzle box: innovative problem-solving by a teleost fish. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 75, 1-11. (doi:10.1007/s00265-020-02953-7) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 41.
Lucon-Xiccato T, Montalbano G, Gatto E, Frigato E, D'Aniello S, Bertolucci C . 2022 Individual differences and knockout in zebrafish reveal similar cognitive effects of BDNF between teleosts and mammals. Proc. R. Soc. B 289, 20222036. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2022.2036) Link, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 42.
Montalbano G, Bertolucci C, Lucon-Xiccato T . 2022 Cognitive phenotypic plasticity: environmental enrichment affects learning but not executive functions in a teleost fish, Poecilia reticulata. Biology 11, 64. (doi:10.3390/biology11010064) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 43.
Lucon-Xiccato T, Bisazza A . 2014 Discrimination reversal learning reveals greater female behavioural flexibility in guppies. Biol. Lett. 10, 20140206. (doi:10.1098/rsbl.2014.0206) Link, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 44.
Lucon-Xiccato T, Bertolucci C . 2019 Guppies show rapid and lasting inhibition of foraging behaviour. Behav. Processes 164, 91-99. (doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2019.04.011) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 45.
Montalbano G, Bertolucci C, Lucon-Xiccato T . 2020 Measures of inhibitory control correlate between different tasks but do not predict problem-solving success in a fish, Poecilia reticulata. Intelligence 82, 101486. (doi:10.1016/j.intell.2020.101486) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 46.
Brown C, Burgess F, Braithwaite VA . 2007 Heritable and experiential effects on boldness in a tropical poeciliid. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 62, 237-243. (doi:10.1007/s00265-007-0458-3) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 47.
Burns JG . 2008 The validity of three tests of temperament in guppies (Poecilia reticulata). J. Comp. Psychol. 122, 344-356. (doi:10.1037/0735-7036.122.4.344) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 48.
Burns JG, Price AC, Thomson JD, Hughes KA, Rodd FH . 2016 Environmental and genetic effects on exploratory behavior of high-and low-predation guppies (Poecilia reticulata). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 70, 1187-1196. (doi:10.1007/s00265-016-2127-x) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 49.
Blaser RE, Chadwick L, McGinnis GC . 2010 Behavioral measures of anxiety in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav. Brain Res. 208, 56-62. (doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2009.11.009) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 50.
Kotrschal A 2014 Artificial selection on relative brain size reveals a positive genetic correlation between brain size and proactive personality in the guppy. Evolution 68, 1139-1149. (doi:10.1111/evo.12341) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 51.
Cattelan S, Lucon-Xiccato T, Pilastro A, Griggio M . 2019 Familiarity mediates equitable social associations in guppies. Behav. Ecol. 30, 249-255. (doi:10.1093/beheco/ary135) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 52.
Kotrschal A, Taborsky B . 2010 Environmental change enhances cognitive abilities in fish. PLoS Biol. 8, e1000351. (doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1000351) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 53.
Lucon-Xiccato T, Montalbano G, Reddon AR, Bertolucci C . 2022 Social environment affects inhibitory control via developmental plasticity in a fish. Anim. Behav. 183, 69-76. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2021.11.001) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 54.
Vila Pouca C, Mitchell DJ, Lefèvre J, Vega-Trejo R, Kotrschal A . 2021 Early predation risk shapes adult learning and cognitive flexibility. Oikos 130, 1477-1486. (doi:10.1111/oik.08481) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 55.
Carbia PS, Brown C . 2020 Seasonal variation of sexually dimorphic spatial learning implicates mating system in the intertidal Cocos Frillgoby (Bathygobius cocosensis). Anim. Cogn. 23, 621-628. (doi:10.1007/s10071-020-01366-3) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 56.
López-Olmeda JF, Zhao H, Reischl M, Pylatiuk C, Lucon-Xiccato T, Loosli F, Foulkes NS . 2021 Long photoperiod impairs learning in male but not female medaka. Iscience 24, 102784. (doi:10.1016/j.isci.2021.102784) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 57.
Ebbesson LOE, Braithwaite VA . 2012 Environmental effects on fish neural plasticity and cognition. J. Fish Biol. 81, 2151-2174. (doi:10.1111/j.1095-8649.2012.03486.x) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 58.
Zupanc GKH . 2006 Neurogenesis and neuronal regeneration in the adult fish brain. J. Comp. Physiol. A 192, 649-670. (doi:10.1007/s00359-006-0104-y) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 59.
Jankowsky JL 2005 Environmental enrichment mitigates cognitive deficits in a mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. J. Neurosci. 25, 5217-5224. (doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5080-04.2005) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 60.
Tang YP, Wang H, Feng R, Kyin M, Tsien JZ . 2001 Differential effects of enrichment on learning and memory function in NR2B transgenic mice. Neuropharmacology 41, 779-790. (doi:10.1016/S0028-3908(01)00122-8) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 61.
Burns JG, Saravanan A, Helen Rodd F . 2009 Rearing environment affects the brain size of guppies: lab-reared guppies have smaller brains than wild-caught guppies. Ethology 115, 122-133. (doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.2008.01585.x) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 62.
Reyes AS, Bittar A, Ávila LC, Botia C, Esmeral NP, Bloch NI . 2022 Divergence in brain size and brain region volumes across wild guppy populations. Proc. R. Soc. B 289, 20212784. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2021.2784) Link, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 63.
Budaev SV, Zhuikov AY . 1998 Avoidance learning and "personality" in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata). J. Comp. Psychol. 112, 92. (doi:10.1037/0735-7036.112.1.92) Crossref, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 64.
Wingfield JC, Kitaysky AS . 2002 Endocrine responses to unpredictable environmental events: stress or anti-stress hormones? Integr. Comp. Biol. 42, 600-609. (doi:10.1093/icb/42.3.600) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 65.
Barreto RE, Volpato GL, Pottinger TG . 2006 The effect of elevated blood cortisol levels on the extinction of a conditioned stress response in rainbow trout. Horm. Behav. 50, 484-488. (doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.06.017) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 66.
Endo Y, Nishimura JI, Kimura F . 1996 Impairment of maze learning in rats following long-term glucocorticoid treatments. Neurosci. Lett. 203, 199-202. (doi:10.1016/0304-3940(95)12296-6) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 67.
Saldanha CJ, Schlinger BA, Clayton NS . 2000 Rapid effects of corticosterone on cache recovery in mountain chickadees (Parus gambeli). Horm. Behav. 37, 109-115. (doi:10.1006/hbeh.2000.1571) Crossref, PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar - 68.
Guidelines for the treatment of animals in behavioural research and teaching . Anim. Behav. 159, I-XI. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2019.11.002) Google Scholar - 69.
Lucon-Xiccato T, Montalbano G, Bertolucci C . 2023Adaptive phenotypic plasticity induces individual variability along a cognitive trade-off . Figshare. (doi:10.6084/m9.figshare.c.6697812) Google Scholar